Info: Zenodo’s user support line is staffed on regular business days between Dec 23 and Jan 5. Response times may be slightly longer than normal.

Published March 29, 2023 | Version 1
Journal article Open

Children learn ergative case marking in Hindi using statistical preemption and clause-level semantics (intentionality): evidence from acceptability judgment and elicited production studies with children and adults

  • 1. School of Health and Social Care,, University of Essex, Colchester, Essex, CO4 3SQ, UK
  • 2. School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol, BS8 1TU, UK
  • 3. Department of Linguistics,, University of Colorado, Boulder, Boulder, Colorado, 80309, USA
  • 4. Language Technologies Research Centre, International Institute of Information Technology-Hyderabad, Gachibowli, Hyderbabad, 500032, India
  • 5. Department of Humanities and Social Sciences, Indian Institute of Technology Delhi, Hauz Khas, New Delhi, 110016, India
  • 6. Department of Computer Science, Ramakrishna Mission Vivekananda Educational and Research Institute, Belur Math, Howrah, West Bengal, 711202, India
  • 7. ESRC International Centre for Language and Communicative Development (LuCiD), International, UK

Description

Background: A question that lies at the very heart of language acquisition research is how children learn semi-regular systems with exceptions (e.g., the English plural rule that yields cats, dogs, etc, with exceptions feet and men). We investigated this question for Hindi ergative ne marking; another semi-regular but exception-filled system. Generally, in the past tense, the subject of two-participant transitive verbs (e.g., Ram broke the cup) is marked with ne, but there are exceptions. How, then, do children learn when ne marking is required, when it is optional, and when it is ungrammatical?

Methods: We conducted two studies using (a) acceptability judgment and (b) elicited production methods with children (aged 4-5, 5-6 and 9-10 years) and adults.

Results: All age groups showed effects of statistical preemption: the greater the frequency with which a particular verb appears with versus without ne marking on the subject – relative to other verbs – the greater the extent to which participants (a) accepted and (b) produced ne over zero-marked subjects. Both children and adults also showed effects of clause-level semantics, showing greater acceptance of ne over zero-marked subjects for intentional than unintentional actions. Some evidence of semantic effects at the level of the verb was observed in the elicited production task for children and the judgment task for adults.

Conclusions: participants mainly learn ergative marking on an input-based verb-by-verb basis (i.e., via statistical preemption; verb-level semantics), but are also sensitive to clause-level semantic considerations (i.e., the intentionality of the action). These findings add to a growing body of work which suggests that children learn semi-regular, exception-filled systems using both statistics and semantics.

Files

openreseurope-3-16872.pdf

Files (2.9 MB)

Name Size Download all
md5:3d22f50cb514f537594b94adbf5b396e
2.9 MB Preview Download

Additional details

References

  • Alishahi A, Stevenson S (2008). A computational model of early argument structure acquisition. Cogn Sci. doi:10.1080/03640210801929287
  • Ambridge B (2013). How do children restrict their linguistic generalizations? An (un-)grammaticality judgment study. Cogn Sci. doi:10.1111/cogs.12018
  • Ambridge B, Ambridge C (2020). The retreat from transitive-causative overgeneralization errors: A review and diary study. Current perspectives on child language acquisition: How children use their environment to learn. doi:10.1075/tilar.27.05amb
  • Ambridge B, Barak L, Wonnacott E (2018). Effects of both preemption and entrenchment in the retreat from verb overgeneralization errors: Four reanalyses, an extended replication, and a meta-analytic synthesis. Collabra Psychol. doi:10.1525/collabra.133
  • Ambridge B, Bidgood A, Twomey KE (2015). Preemption versus Entrenchment: Towards a Construction-General Solution to the Problem of the Retreat from Verb Argument Structure Overgeneralization. PLoS One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0123723
  • Ambridge B, Blything RP (2016). A connectionist model of the retreat from verb argument structure overgeneralization. J Child Lang. doi:10.1017/S0305000915000586
  • Ambridge B, Brandt S (2013). Lisa filled water into the cup: The roles of entrenchment, preemption and verb semantics in German speakers' L2 acquisition of English locatives. Zeitschrift für Anglistik und Amerikanistik. doi:10.1515/zaa-2013-0304
  • Ambridge B, Doherty L, Maitreyee R (2022). Testing a computational model of causative overgeneralizations: Child judgment and production data from English, Hebrew, Hindi, Japanese and K'iche' [version 2; peer review: 2 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. Open Res Eur. doi:10.12688/openreseurope.13008.2
  • Ambridge B, Maitreyee R, Saxena G (2023a). Ergative marking in Hindi: Stimuli, data and R code. Open Science Framework.
  • Ambridge B, Maitreyee R, Narasimhan B (2023b). CLASS: Cross linguistic acquisition of sentence structure. Open Science Framework.
  • Ambridge B, Maitreyee R, Tatsumi T (2020). The crosslinguistic acquisition of sentence structure: Computational modeling and grammaticality judgments from adult and child speakers of English, Japanese, Hindi, Hebrew and K'iche'. Cognition. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104310
  • Ambridge B, Pine J, Rowland C (2011). Children use verb semantics to retreat from overgeneralization errors: A novel verb grammaticality judgment study. Cognitive Linguistics. doi:10.1515/cogl.2011.012
  • Ambridge B, Pine JM, Rowland CF (2012a). Semantics versus statistics in the retreat from locative overgeneralization errors. Cognition. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.01.002
  • Ambridge B, Pine JM, Rowland CF (2009). A semantics-based approach to the "no negative evidence" problem. Cogn Sci. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01055.x
  • Ambridge B, Pine JM, Rowland CF (2014). Avoiding dative overgeneralisation errors: semantics, statistics or both?. Lang Cogn Neurosci. doi:10.1080/01690965.2012.738300
  • Ambridge B, Pine JM, Rowland CF (2008). The effect of verb semantic class and verb frequency (entrenchment) on children's and adults' graded judgements of argument-structure overgeneralization errors. Cognition. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.015
  • Ambridge B, Pine JM, Rowland CF (2012b). The roles of verb semantics, entrenchment, and morphophonology in the retreat from dative argument-structure overgeneralization errors. Language. doi:10.1353/lan.2012.0000
  • Ambridge B, Pine JM, Rowland CF (2013). The retreat from overgeneralization in child language acquisition: Word learning, morphology, and verb argument structure. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. doi:10.1002/wcs.1207
  • (2016). Audacity(R): Free Audio Editor and Recorder (Version 2.1.2) [Computer application].
  • Barak L, Goldberg AE, Stevenson S (2016). Comparing computational cognitive models of generalization in a language acquisition task. Proceedings of the 2016 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing. doi:10.18653/v1/D16-1010
  • Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Bhat RA, Bhat IA, Sharma DM (2017). Improving transition-based dependency parsing of Hindi and Urdu by modeling syntactically relevant phenomena. ACM transactions on Asian and low-resource language information processing. doi:10.1145/3005447
  • Bidgood A, Ambridge B, Pine JM (2014). The retreat from locative overgeneralisation errors: a novel verb grammaticality judgment study. PLoS One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0097634
  • Blything RP, Ambridge B, Lieven EV (2014). Children use statistics and semantics in the retreat from overgeneralization. PLoS One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0110009
  • Bowerman M (1988). The "no negative evidence" problem: How do children avoid constructing an overly general grammar?. Explaining language universals.
  • Boyd JK, Goldberg AE (2011). Learning what NOT to say: The role of statistical a. preemption and categorization in -adjective production. Language. doi:10.1353/lan.2011.0012
  • Brooks PJ, Tomasello M, Dodson K (1999). Young children's overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity verbs. Child Dev. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00097
  • Brooks PJ, Zizak O (2002). Does preemption help children learn verb transitivity?. J Child Lang. doi:10.1017/s0305000902005287
  • Butt M, King TH (1991). Semantic case in Urdu. Papers from the 27th regional meeting of the Chicago linguistic society.
  • Butt M, King TH (2003). Case systems: Beyond structural distinctions. New perspectives on Case Theory.
  • de Hoop H, Narasimhan B (2005). Differential case-marking in Hindi.
  • Goldberg AE (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure.
  • Goldberg AE (2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language.
  • Goldberg AE (2011). Corpus evidence of the viability of statistical preemption. Cogn Linguist. doi:10.1515/cogl.2011.006
  • Goldberg AE (2019). Explain me this: Creativity, competition, and the partial productivity of constructions. doi:10.2307/j.ctvc772nn
  • (2022). 2 years of COVID-19 on GOV.UK.
  • Gropen J, Pinker S, Hollander M (1991). Affectedness and direct objects: The role of lexical semantics in the acquisition of verb argument structure. Cognition. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(91)90035-3
  • Gropen J, Pinker S, Hollander M (1989). The learnability and acquisition of the dative alternation in English. Language. doi:10.2307/415332
  • Harmon Z, Kapatsinski V (2017). Putting old tools to novel uses: The role of form accessibility in semantic extension. Cogn Psychol. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.08.002
  • Hopper PJ, Thompson SA (1980). Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language. doi:10.2307/413757
  • Hsu AS, Chater N (2010). The logical problem of language acquisition: A probabilistic perspective. Cogn Sci. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01117.x
  • Irani A (2009). How children learn to disappear causative errors.
  • Kakwani D, Kunchukuttan A, Golla S (2020). IndicNLPSuite: Monolingual corpora, evaluation benchmarks and pre-trained multilingual language models for Indian languages. doi:10.18653/v1/2020.findings-emnlp.445
  • Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. J Stat Softw. doi:10.18637/jss.v082.i13
  • Li P, MacWhinney B (1996). Cryptotype, overgeneralization and competition: A connectionist model of the learning of English reversive prefixes. Conn Sci. doi:10.1080/095400996116938
  • List JM, Cysouw M, Forkel R (2016). Concepticon: A resource for the linking of concept lists.
  • Mohanan T (1994). Argument structure in Hindi.
  • Montaut A (2013). The rise of non-canonical subjects and semantic alignments in Hindi. Stud Lang Companion Ser. doi:10.1075/slcs.140.05mon
  • Marcus G, Davis E (2019). Rebooting AI: Building artificial intelligence we can trust.
  • Narasimhan B (2005). Splitting the notion of 'agent': Case-marking in early child Hindi. J Child Lang. doi:10.1017/S0305000905007117
  • Pareek B, Kidwai A, Eisenbeiss S (2016). Verb agreement in Hindi and its acquisition.
  • Peirce J, Gray JR, Simpson S (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behav Res Methods. doi:10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y
  • Perek F, Goldberg AE (2017). Linguistic generalization on the basis of function and constraints on the basis of statistical preemption. Cognition. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.019
  • Perfors A, Tenenbaum JB, Wonnacott E (2010). Variability, negative evidence, and the acquisition of verb argument constructions. J Child Lang. doi:10.1017/S0305000910000012
  • Piepers J (2016). Optional ergative case marking in Hindi.
  • Pinker S (1989). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure.
  • Pinker S (1999). Words and rules: The ingredients of language.
  • (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing.
  • Robenalt C, Goldberg AE (2015). Judgment evidence for statistical preemption: It is relatively better to vanish than to disappear a rabbit, but a lifeguard can equally well backstroke or children to shore. Cogn Linguist. doi:10.1515/cog-2015-0004
  • Robenalt C, Goldberg AE (2016). Nonnative speakers do not take competing alternative expressions into account the way native speakers do. Lang Learn. doi:10.1111/lang.12149
  • Rosenthal R (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol Bull. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638
  • Shibatani M, Pardeshi P (2002). The causative continuum. doi:10.1075/tsl.48.07shi
  • Singh M (1998). On the semantics of the perfective aspect. Nat Lang Semant.
  • Stefanowitsch A (2008). Negative entrenchment: A usage-based approach to negative evidence. Cogn Linguist. doi:10.1515/COGL.2008.020
  • Theakston AL (2004). The role of entrenchment in children's and adults' performance on grammaticality judgment tasks. Cogn Dev. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.08.001
  • Twomey KE, Chang F, Ambridge B (2014). Do as I say, not as I do: a lexical distributional account of English locative verb class acquisition. Cogn Psychol. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.05.001
  • Twomey KE, Chang F, Ambridge B (2016). Lexical distributional cues, but not situational cues, are readily used to learn abstract locative verb-structure associations. Cognition. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.001
  • (2021). Coronavirus: A history of English lockdown laws.
  • Wonnacott E, Newport EL, Tanenhaus MK (2008). Acquiring and processing verb argument structure: distributional learning in a miniature language. Cogn Psychol. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.04.002