Planned intervention: On Wednesday June 26th 05:30 UTC Zenodo will be unavailable for 10-20 minutes to perform a storage cluster upgrade.
Published July 15, 2021 | Version 1
Journal article Open

Naïve, unenculturated chimpanzees fail to make and use flaked stone tools

  • 1. Department of Early Prehistory and Quaternary Ecology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, 72070, Germany
  • 2. Department of Human Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, 04103, Germany
  • 3. Kristiansand Zoo, Kardemomme By, Kristiansand, 4609, Norway
  • 4. Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary Synthesis, University of Oslo, Oslo, NO-0316, Norway

Description

Background: Despite substantial research on early hominin lithic technologies, the learning mechanisms underlying flake manufacture and use are contested. To draw phylogenetic inferences on the potential cognitive processes underlying the acquisition of both of these abilities in early hominins, we investigated if and how one of our closest living relatives, chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes), could learn to make and use flakes.

Methods: Across several experimental conditions, we tested eleven task-naïve chimpanzees (unenculturated n=8, unknown status n=3) from two independent populations for their abilities to spontaneously make and subsequently use flakes as well as to use flakes made by a human experimenter.

Results: Despite the fact that the chimpanzees seemed to understand the requirements of the task, were sufficiently motivated and had ample opportunities to develop the target behaviours, none of the chimpanzees tested made or used flakes in any of the experimental conditions.

Conclusions: These results differ from all previous ape flaking experiments, which found flake manufacture and use in bonobos and one orangutan. However, these earlier studies tested human-enculturated apes and provided test subjects with flake making and using demonstrations. The contrast between these earlier positive findings and our negative findings (despite using a much larger sample size) suggests that enculturation and/or demonstrations may be necessary for chimpanzees to acquire these abilities. The data obtained in this study are consistent with the hypothesis that flake manufacture and use might have evolved in the hominin lineage after the split between Homo and Pan 7 million years ago, a scenario further supported by the initial lack of flaked stone tools in the archaeological record after this split. We discuss possible evolutionary scenarios for flake manufacture and use in both non-hominin and hominin lineages.

Files

openreseurope-1-15034.pdf

Files (1.0 MB)

Name Size Download all
md5:7e32fb17dbbaed7dd73c24c462459080
1.0 MB Preview Download

Additional details

References

  • Archer W, Aldeias A, McPherron SP (2020). What is 'in situ'? a reply to Harmand (2015). J Hum Evol. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2020.102740
  • Arroyo A, Hirata S, Matsuzawa T (2016). Nut cracking tools used by captive chimpanzees ( ) and their comparison with early stone age percussive artefacts from Olduvai Gorge. PLoS One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0166788
  • Bandini E, Motes-Rodrigo A, Steele MP (2020). Examining the mechanisms underlying the acquisition of animal tool behaviour. Biol Lett. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2020.0122
  • Biro D, Inoue-Nakamura N, Tonooka R (2003). Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments. Anim Cogn. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0183-x
  • Boesch-Achermann H, Boesch C (1993). Tool use in wild chimpanzees: New light from dark forests. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770551
  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures.
  • Braun DR, Aldeias V, Archer W (2019). Earliest known Oldowan artifacts at >2.58 Ma from Ledi-Geraru, Ethiopia, highlight early technological diversity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. doi:10.1073/pnas.1820177116
  • Brunon A, Bovet D, Bourgeois A (2014). Motivation and manipulation capacities of the blue and yellow macaw and the tufted capuchin: A comparative approach. Behav Processes. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.06.006
  • Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Call J (2007). Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Dev Sci. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00630.x
  • Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Call J (2013). Chimpanzees, , recognize successful actions, but fail to imitate them. Anim Behav. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.015
  • Byrne RW, Russon AE (1998). Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach. Behav Brain Sci. doi:10.1017/s0140525x98001745
  • Carvalho S, Cunha E, Sousa C (2008). Chaînes opératoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee ( ) nut cracking. J Hum Evol. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.02.005
  • Carvalho S, McGrew WC (2012). The origins of the Oldowan: why chimpanzees ( ) still are good models for technological evolution in Africa. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139149327.010
  • Clay Z, Tennie C (2018). Is overimitation a uniquely human phenomenon? insights from human children as compared to bonobos. Child Dev. doi:10.1111/cdev.12857
  • Cohen J (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. doi:10.4324/9780203771587
  • Corbey R, Jagich A, Vaesen K (2016). The Acheulean handaxe: more like a bird's song than a beatles' tune?. Evol Anthropol. doi:10.1002/evan.21467
  • Domínguez-Rodrigo M, Alcalá L (2016). 3.3-million-year-old stone tools and butchery traces? more evidence needed. PaleoAnthropology.
  • Eren MI, Lycett SJ, Tomonaga M (2020). Underestimating Kanzi? Exploring Kanzi-Oldowan comparisons in light of recent human stone tool replication. Evol Anthropol. doi:10.1002/evan.21858
  • Foley R (1987). Hominid species and stone-tool assemblages: how are they related?. Antiquity. doi:10.1017/S0003598X00072938
  • Forss S, Motes-Rodrigo A, Hrubesch C (2020). Chimpanzees' ( ) problem-solving skills are influenced by housing facility and captive care duration. PeerJ. doi:10.7717/peerj.10263
  • Furlong EE, Boose KJ, Boysen ST (2008). Raking it in: the impact of enculturation on chimpanzee tool use. Anim Cogn. doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0091-6
  • Gruber T (2016). Great apes do not learn novel tool use easily: Conservatism, functional fixedness, or cultural influence?. Int J Primatol. doi:10.1007/s10764-016-9902-4
  • Harmand S, Lewis JE, Feibel CS (2015). 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature. doi:10.1038/nature14464
  • Hayashi M (2015). Perspectives on object manipulation and action grammar for percussive actions in primates. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. doi:10.1098/rstb.2014.0350
  • Henrich J, Tennie C (2017). Cultural evolution in chimpanzees and humans. doi:10.4159/9780674982642-018
  • Hopper LM, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ (2008). Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through 'ghost' conditions. Proc Biol Sci. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1542
  • Hopper LM, Spiteri A, Lambeth SP (2007). Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees. Anim Behav. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.07.016
  • Mercader J, Barton H, Gillespie J (2007). 4,300-Year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. doi:10.1073/pnas.0607909104
  • Mercader J, Panger M, Boesch C (2002). Excavation of a chimpanzee stone tool site in the African rainforest. Science. doi:10.1126/science.1070268
  • Mora R, de la Torre I (2005). Percussion tools in Olduvai Beds I and II (Tanzania): implications for early human activities. J Anthropol Archaeol. doi:10.1016/j.jaa.2004.12.001
  • Motes-Rodrigo A, Bandini E, Archer W (2021). Naïve, unenculturated chimpanzees fail to make and use flaked stone tools.
  • (2011). Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals.
  • Panger MA, Brooks AS, Richmond BG (2002). Older than the Oldowan? rethinking the emergence of hominin tool use. Evol Anthropol. doi:10.1002/evan.10094
  • Pope SM, Taglialatela JP, Skiba SA (2018). Changes in frontoparietotemporal connectivity following do-as-i-do imitation training in chimpanzees ( ). J Cogn Neurosci. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01217
  • Proffitt T, Luncz LV, Falótico T (2016). Wild monkeys flake stone tools. Nature. doi:10.1038/nature20112
  • Roche H (2000). Variability of Pliocene lithic productions in East Africa. Acta Anthropol Sin.
  • Rolian C, Carvalho S (2017). Tool use and manufacture in the last common ancestor of Pan and Homo. doi:10.4159/9780674982642-017
  • Savage-Rumbaugh S, Fields WM (2006). Rules and tools: beyond anthropomorphism.
  • Schick KD, Toth NP (1994). Making silent stones speak: human evolution and the dawn of technology.
  • Schick KD, Toth N, Garufi G (1999). Continuing investigations into the stone tool-making and tool-using capabilities of a bonobo ( ). J Archaeol Sci. doi:10.1006/jasc.1998.0350
  • Semaw S, Renne P, Harris JW (1997). 2.5-million-year-old stone tools from Gona, Ethiopia. Nature. doi:10.1038/385333a0
  • Stout D, Rogers MJ, Jaeggi AV (2019). Archaeology and the origins of human cumulative culture: a case study from the earliest Oldowan at Gona, Ethiopia. Curr Anthropol. doi:10.1086/703173
  • Suddendorf T, Whiten A (2001). Mental evolution and development: evidence for secondary representation in children, great ages, and other animals. Psychol Bull. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.5.629
  • Tennie C (2019). Could nonhuman great apes also have cultural evolutionary psychology?. Behav Brain Sci. doi:10.1017/S0140525X19001055
  • Tennie C, Braun DR, Premo LS (2016). The Island test for cumulative culture in the Paleolithic. The Nature of Culture. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-7426-0_11
  • Tennie C, Bandini E, van Schaik CP (2020a). The zone of latent solutions and its relevance to understanding ape cultures. Biol Philos. doi:10.1007/s10539-020-09769-9
  • Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0052
  • Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2012). Untrained chimpanzees ( ) fail to imitate novel actions. PLoS One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041548
  • Tennie C, Hopper L, van Schaik CP (2020b). On the origin of cumulative culture: consideration of the role of copying in culture-dependent traits and a reappraisal of the zone of latent solutions hypothesis.
  • Tennie C, Premo LS, Braun DR (2017). Early stone tools and cultural transmission: resetting the null hypothesis. Curr Anthropol. doi:10.1086/693846
  • Tomasello M, Call J (1997). Primate cognition.
  • Tomasello M, Savage-Rumbaugh S, Kruger AC (1993). Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Dev. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb04207.x
  • Toth N, Schick K, Semaw S (2006). A comparative study of the stone tool-making skills of , , and .
  • Toth N, Schick KD, Savage-Rumbaugh ES (1993). the tool-maker: investigations into the stone tool-making and tool-using capabilities of a bonobo ( ). J Archaeol Sci. doi:10.1006/jasc.1993.1006
  • Westergaard GC, Suomi SJ (1994). A simple stone-tool technology in monkeys. J Hum Evol. doi:10.1006/jhev.1994.1055
  • Westergaard GC, Suomi SJ (1995). The stone tools of capuchins. Int J Primatol. doi:10.1007/BF02696114
  • Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew WC (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature. doi:10.1038/21415
  • Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew WC (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour. doi:10.1163/156853901317367717
  • Whiten A, Horner V, de Waal FBM (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature. doi:10.1038/nature04047
  • Whiten A, Spiteri A, Horner V (2007). Transmission of multiple traditions within and between chimpanzee groups. Curr Biol. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.031
  • Wright RVS (1972). Imitative learning of a flaked stone technology-the case of an orangutan. Mankind. doi:10.1111/j.1835-9310.1972.tb00451.x
  • Wu C (2011). VisualSFM: a visual structure from motion system.
  • Wynn T, Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Marchant LF (2011). "An ape's view of the Oldowan" revisited. Evol Anthropol. doi:10.1002/evan.20323