•INSTITUT imtnnRiiuimu I D ' O . S- T |No CM •:• H$ INTERNATIONAL LABOUR OFFICE STUDIES AND REPORTS Séries J (Education) No. 3 MOTION STUDY IN TYPEWRITING A RECORD OF EXPERIMENTS BY J. M. LAHY Head of the Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, Ecole des Hautes Etudes, Paris GENEVA 1924 PREFATORY NOTE The report on ability in typewriting published by the International Labour Office in October 1923 gave a practical example of the application of experimental methods to vocational guidance. The author, Mademoiselle Dora Bieneman, gave first a brief analysis of the work of typewriting, and then endeavoured to arrive at methods of detecting the abilities required. Her report both emphasised the complexity of the problem and described the mechanism of tests. The present report is of quite a different character. The author set out to discover by scientific investigation the best methods for typewriting. Hitherto the technique of typewriting has been mainly a matter of rule of thumb. Mr. Lahy, on the other hand, in a series of experiments conducted from 1912 to 1914, and by control experiments since the war, made a detailed investigation of the mechanism of touch, and he hopes to arrive at genuinely scientific rules by graphic motion study. As a result of his experiments he draws many conclusions relating to teaching which should be of value, not only in the selection and training of typists, but also in constructing a scientific keyboard. The value of the investigations, however, does not end with these practical applications. .Mr. Lahy does not pretend to give a final answer to every prol k m which arises. His object is rather, as he says himself, to m ike known " a method of studying occupational problems". It is indeed a problem of methods of organisation which is involved, going beyond the boundaries of the occupation of typewriting and applying to almost every form of human work. In the words of the author, " co-operation 1 Ability in Typewriting in Relation to Vocational Guidance, by Mademoiselle Dora B I E N E M \ N . S¡ . I tics and Reporls, Series J (Education), No. 2. Geneva, 1923, 47 pp. s.; 25 cents. — 4 — between manufacturers and scientists should become the basic principle of social progress". Mr. Lahy's work has thus a very wide application, since it gives a concrete illustration of the manner in which psycho-physiology can be of service to industry. This, as much as the strict scientific accuracy of the experiments described, is the reason for which it is now published. - 5 — CONTENTS Page INTRODUCTION. I. Technical Progress and Scientific Method . . . M E T H O D OF R E C O R D I N G Pneumatic Recording Electric Recording Electric Recording Electric Recording Electric Recording Combined Methods of II. III. IV. V. by Spring Contacts by Mercury Contacts by Contact with the Escapement . . Recording 7 11 11 13 14 15 16 16 CHOICE OF SUBJECTS AND T E S T 19 ANALYSIS OF THE GRAPHS 21 RESULTS 22 Period of Depression of Keys Duration of Intervals. Necessity of Alternating the Hands Confirmation of Rule of Alternation. Non-Alternating Phrases and Isolated Letters Sentence Typed with the Right H a n d Sentence Typed with the Left Hand Typing Two Isolated Letters Typing Short Isolated Words Relation between Period of Contacts, Period of Intervals, and Force of Stroke Physiological Reasons for Alternation of the Hands . . . . Limits to the Rule of Alternation Use of Fingers of One Hand. " J u m p i n g " and Alternation R h y t h m in Typewriting 22 27 31 31 32 33 34 36 39 51 51 53 T H E TEACHING OF T Y P E W R I T I N G 57 Selection of Typists and Methods of Training Scientific Arrangement of the Keyboard 57 58 (i TABLES I. II. III. IV. V. VI. VII. VIII. IX. X. XI. XII. XIII. XIV. XV. Period of contacts and intervals, graph 84 Period of contacts and intervals, graph 224 Relative period of depression of different keys Duration of strokes with the left and right hand respectively Duration of strokes and intervals for four subjects . . Analysis of graphs of the touch of good typists Results of typing sentence with the right hand only . . . Results of typing sentence with the left hand only. . . . Results of striking isolated keys Results of typing isolated words Variations in speed index, contacts, and intervals, with complete absence of alternation (test 1), occasional alternation (test 2), and equal distribution of alternation and non-alternation (test 3) Results of tapping tests Relation between period of contacts and intervals with and without alternation of the hands Results of " j u m p i n g " and alternating fingers of the same hand . .' Average deviation of alternating intervals Page 24 25 26 26 28 30 32 33 34 35 37 42 47 52 54 FIGURES 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. Diagram of apparatus for pneumatic recording Marey drum Desprez recorder The first Desprez recorder (on the right) records strokes electrically, the second (in the middle) records hundredths of seconds, and the Marey drum records strokes pneumatically Diagram of appliance for electric recording by spring contacts Diagram of contacts for electric recording by mercury contacts 'Map ' typewriter fitted for electric and pneumatic recording Graphic record of the sentence: Nous sommes en possession General view of the apparatus Diagram showing numerical results of graphic record of one subject's strokes Graphic record of the word monsieur Distribution curve of reaction times, subject A Distribution curve of reaction times, subject B Chronological record of reaction times, subject A Chronological record of reaction times, subject B Page 11 12 13 14 14 15 17 18 20 29 38 49 49 50 50 INTRODUCTION Technical Progress and Scientific Method The efforts which men have made throughout the centuries to perfect the technique of the ancient trades almost pass the imagination. Innumerable blind attempts, experience lost and regained, sage observations forgotten and repeatedly revived, have gone to build up the tradition of the carpenter's or blacksmith's movements, and make of it a rule of work which is religiously handed on from master to apprentice. In primitive civilisations the technique of many trades, as sociology and ethnology have proved, had their origin in magical beliefs. The effects of movements which are today attributed to the. physical constitution of the worker were believed by primitive man to be the work of a supernatural power in the tools themselves. The primitive beliefs of the first artisans have given its strength to the tradition which compels a young worker to adopt the movements evolved by age-long experience. Excellent though the final results of such methods may be, it is inconceivable that they should be applied to the many new callings which have appeared in modern society. Nevertheless, one of the newest occupations, that of typewriting, followed this outworn method until psycho-physiology took its rightful place in the sphere of human labour and offered, in place of the primitive rule of thumb, a technique based on scientific study of the movements required for the typewriter. This is as yet still a recent development, as to the writer's knpwledge his are the first experimental investigations undertaken. Although typewriting is barely forty years old, it has no rules of work except those due to haphazard individual discoveries, sometimes handed on, more often lost, and in no case absolutely — 8 — valid, as they were not based on scientific investigation. The keyboard, for instance, was invented in America for purposes of correspondence in the English language; the letters are grouped, according to the frequency with which they occur, on those portions of the keyboard which seem easiest to reach. The frequency with which letters occur varies, however, in different languages; and thus English-speaking typists are exceptionally favoured in comparison with their colleagues in other parts of the world. But there is a still more striking fact to be noted: as the letters were arranged solely according to their frequency of occurrence, no attempt was made to determine their relation to the ease or otherwise of different finger movements. No one tried to discover the easiest combination of movements in order to use them for the combinations of letters which occur most frequently. This study will show that the so-called "universal keyboard " is not really the best, even for Americans who gave it to the world. In trying to frame rules for teaching typewriting, false conceptions of human work were adopted. Instead of utilising observation and experiment, the so-called "ten-finger method " was imposed on all pupils, regardless of the fact that this method, although excellent for exercising the muscles of the hand, had absolutely no experimental basis and was in fact used by a very limited number of persons only. Needless to say, skilled psychophysiologists were never invited to assist by studying the movements involved in order to determine scientifically the laws governing the adaptation of movements to the purposes of the occupation, i. e. accuracy and rapidity. The legitimate criticisms of the ten-finger method subsequently to be advanced show the utility of rules based on graphic analysis of movements. Readers of this study should certainly be convinced that by the adoption of scientific methods humanity could gain years and even centuries in establishing the best rules for executing occupational work. Co-operation between manufacturers and scientists should become the basic principle of social progress. The experiments described in this volume were begun in 1912 and continued until 1914, when the results now published had already been obtained. They were not then made public, as it was desired to carry out a series of control experiments with — 9 — professional typists whose technical opinions did not agree with these results. The control experiments were resumed after the war. In July 1921, for example, the presence in Paris of a large number of first-class typists who were competing in the international championship organised by the Chamber of Commercial Organisation (Chambre syndicale d'organisation commerciale) offered an opportunity for further experiments. The time is now ripe for publishing the results of these experiments, with the certainty that they will provide professional typists with positive data enabling them to improve or modify their methods of work. I Method of Recording The method employed for analysing touch was to record the movements of the keys of a typewriter when struck by typists of all grades of skill; these movements are measured by the graphic method usually employed in physiological laboratories. Three methods of transmitting the movements of the keys to a recording cylinder were employed, and will be described in succession. PNEUMATIC RECORDING As the keys of a typewriter are rigidly fixed to the levers (key bars) which move the type bars, a graph of the striking movements can be obtained by recording the movements described at any point on the mechanism. The. point chosen was towards the front of the key bar, which is depressed when the key is struck. d / TTTTTTTTTTTT t c s c FIG. 1. Diagram of apparatus for pneumatic recording. In the pneumatic mechanism adopted (Fig. 1), a flexible indiarubber tube t, sufficiently rigid to revert immediately to its normal position when compressed by a blow from a hard — 12 — body, is fixed under the machine. This tube is held in position, perpendicular to the key bars, by a metal trough c, c, c. At one side of this trough there is a hole through which passes a fine indiarubber tube. The other end of the tube is connected to a Marey drum (Fig. 2). The Marey drum, or lever drum, consists FIG. 2. Marey drum. of a metal cupola, the mouth of which is covered by a thin sheet of rubber m. On the centre of the rubber sheet is fixed a very light metal disc d, carrying a lever hinged at a which causes a light pen or needle inserted at p to oscillate about a fixed point /. The movements of this pen are registered on a revolving cylinder e (Figs. 1 and 4). The whole apparatus is air-tight, the air inside it being maintained at atmospheric pressure by a valve s (Fig. 1). When the typist strikes a key, the key bar is depressed and comes into contact with the tube t. This drives the air towards the Marey drum and distends the rubber sheet. A movement is thus imparted to the recording needle, causing it to deviate from the position of equilibrium as long as the key bar presses on the tube. When this contact ceases the needle returns to its original position. As each key is struck, the corresponding letter is marked on a sheet of paper inserted in the machine, and simultaneously the duration and range of the lever's movements are marked on the recording cylinder. The range is measured by the distance from its original position traversed by the recording needle, and the duration by the time which elapses between the departure of the pen from and its return to its original position. — 13 The duration of the movements can be measured by recording time divisions simultaneously. There are several methods of doing this. The electric diapason was chosen for the present experiment, as it gives absolutely accurate readings if the instrument is graduated. The diapason, for instance, gives 100 double vibrations per second; each vibration makes and breaks an electric circuit, and the current synchronously moves the armature of an electro-magnet (Fig. 3) in a regular rhythm FIG. 3. Desprez recorder. of 100 movements per second. A recording needle fixed to the armature impinges on the recording cylinder parallel to the pen of the lever drum and on the same generator of the cylinder (Fig. 5). ELECTRIC RECORDING If it is desired to study the duration of each striking movement without regard to the force employed, electric recording is preferable. The typewriter is placed in an electric circuit, with a battery and a Desprez recorder. A movable part of the typewriter, set in motion every time the typist strikes the keys, acts as circuit breaker, its movements being transmitted to the recorder which registers them on the cylinder. The problem of electric recording is therefore to find what part of the machine can be used as circuit breaker when the keys are struck. Three different systems were used: spring contacts; mercury contacts; contacts on the escapement. — 14 — FIG. 4. The first Desprez recorder (on the right) records strokes electrically, the second (in the middle) records hundredths of seconds, and the Marey drum records strokes pneumatically. Electric Recording bij Spring Contacts. In the first system a horizontal metal rod, resting on two small hollow supports, is placed under the key bars of the typewriter and at right angles to them (Fig. 5). A vertical rod t, sliding FIG. 5. Diagram ot appliance lor electric recordingby spring contacts. in each of the supports P, with as light friction as possible, is connected with the end of the horizontal metal rod T at A. A spiral spring in each support keeps the rod at a fixed level corresponding to the spot where each key bar touches the horizontal rod. The apparatus is fixed to the typewriter, but is — 15 — insulated from it by sheets of ebonite, so that, when the machine and the apparatus are placed in an electric circuit, it is closed every time a key is struck and the corresponding key bar comes into contact with the horizontal rod. This rod must be perfectly rigid, to avoid accidental contacts through vibrations caused by the violent shock imparted to the keys when struck. The springs in the two supports ease the working of the apparatus, while ensuring permanent contact. The body of the machine is connected to one of the poles of an electric battery. The horizontal rod is connected to the other in such a manner that the circuit is closed when the key bar comes into contact with the horizontal rod. A Desprez recorder similar to that described above for measuring time is placed in the circuit; and each contact is thus registered on the recording cylinder. It was found that this arrangement did not hamper typists in their work, and proved satisfactory to the subjects examined, although they were, very particular as to the adjustment of their machines. Electric Recording by Mercury Contacts. This system was employed until 1914. After the war, however, as the investigations progressed, it was desired to repeat the experiments and make them more accurate. A new mercury contact appliance was adopted. A tank of mercury B, 26 by 4 centimetres, was placed under the key bars of the machine and at right angles to them (Fig. 6). Needles of galvanised brass were attached to each key bar in the same vertical plane, almost touching the surface of the mercury. ^ \ B FIG. 6. Diagram of contacts for electric recording by mercury contacts. The machine as a whole is connected with one pole of an electric battery and the mercury in the tank with the other. — 16 — When a key is struck, the point of the needle enters the mercury and closes the circuit, a Desprez recorder registering the breaking and closing of the circuit. Here also the time was recorded in hundredths of a second at the same moment and on the same generator of the recording cylinder. The duration of each stroke and the interval between strokes could thus be measured automatically. with great accuracy. The very accuracy of the apparatus, however, rendered it difficult to operate. It was necessary to adjust the level of the mercury so that the keys most rapidly struck did not overlap, even when the operator was an extremely rapid typist. Once this adjustment had been made, however, it suited all the subjects. It was also necessary to ensure that the duration of contact was the same for every stroke of equal duration. To ensure this result, the points of all the needles were carefully fixed in the same horizontal plane, and the level of the mercury and the body of the machine itself were both kept perfectly horizontal. The time required to lower the point of the needle to the level of the mercury was insignificant, a space of one or two millimetres being sufficient for adjustment. Electric Recording by Contact with the Escapement. As machines of different makes were used in the investigations a simpler apparatus was adopted. The movements of all the keys of a typewriter are transmitted to the escapement, which then moves the carriage. This part of the machine is easily accessible, and is subject to alternating movements corresponding to the movements of the keys. A record of the electric contacts effected by this part of a typewriter therefore reproduces the movements of the keys. C O M B I N E D M E T H O D S OY R E C O R D I N G The escapement can also be used for pneumatic recording. For this purpose all that is needed is to place the end of the lever of a Marey drum in contact with the movable portion; this — 17 — drum is connected with a second one placed on the recording cylinder, so that all the movements of the escapement are transmitted to the needle of the cylinder. On the French ' Map ' typewriter, now being used for the investigations, another part of the machine, the universal bar, was selected, as the range of its movements is greater and consequently easier to record. A lever drum, placed under the machine, receives the movements of this bar and transmits them to the recording drum. As the space bar does not move in relation with the universal bar, another lever drum is adjusted to it, and connected with the other drums; its movements are thus transmitted to the recording drum (Fig. 7). F I G . 7. ' Map * typewriter fitted for pneumatic and electric recording. A typist's stroke, recorded pneumatically and electrically, and the time in hundredths of a second, can thus be recorded on the same chart (Fig. 8) and analysed with great accuracy. The machines used for the experiment were a Smith Brothers No. 5, a Remington with non-visible writing, one of the latest Remington models, and the French Map machine, with which experiments are at present being pursued. The graphs reproduced in this volume were all made with Map machines. 2 ^í^ 1 2 3 1 2 3 F I G . 8. Grapiiic record of the sentence: Nous sommes en possession... (1) Record of strokes b y p n e u m a t i c transmission (Marey d r u m ) . (2) Record of hundredths of seconds (electro-diapason and Desprez recorder). (3) Record of strokes by electric transmission (Desprez recorder). — 19 — The apparatus as a whole is shown in figure 9, where the various parts described can easily be recognised: recording cylinder, recording signals and drums; and the - typewriter equipped for the experiment. II Choice of Subjects and Test The persons and the test to be employed for the experiments were carefully selected. The typists were all professionals of the highest class, as the difficult and obscure problem of the abilities of average workers and the conditions required for doing good work can only be solved by studying the movements of the best workers. It was essential that all the subjects should have sufficient experience to type an ordinary sentence automatically. Several sentences of this kind were used, but as all the results agreed those given here are all based on one sentence : Nous sommes en possession de votre lettre (We are in receipt of your letter), in order to limit this study to essentials. This test sentence was chosen in order to obviate any mental effort, and in fact none of the subjects paid any attention to his movements. Most of them replied, when questioned concerning the test : " My fingers hit the keys of their own accord ", and they all stated that they typed it absolutely mechanically. Some even said that they "thought on the machine", presumably meaning that their mental images were exclusively those of typing movements. and not those of handwriting. A psychological examination of this kind was necessary to ensure that all the subjects could be compared with each other; otherwise the experiment would have had to be carried out on completely different lines. Another method (I have recently adopted it for further experiments, the results of which will be published separately) would have been to follow the progress made by one subject during the course of his training, with a view to ascertaining the aptitudes most easily acquired and his mental losses, or again to take subjects who had attained F I G . 9. General view of the a p p a r a t u s . — 21 — varying degrees of skill according to their period of training but had all been trained in the same way. Of all these methods the one provisionally selected was that which seemed best calculated to give general results. All the subjects seemed to be keenly interested in the investigations and took an evident pleasure in submitting to the experiments, thus making it possible to obtain a considerable number of graphs, which constitute important documentary evidence. Typists at the top of the profession very kindly gave up their time and spent long days in the laboratory, then at some distance from Paris. They made valuable observations on their individual technique, the analysis of their movements, sensations, and ideas. They thus provided material for extensive psycho-physiological research and opened up hitherto unexplored spheres. Mr. Charles Dellion proved a valuable and untiring collaborator. He was present at many of the experiments and used the results in working out his method of typewriting 1 . This method constitutes the first attempt at adapting the data of experimental science to professional technique. It will mark an epoch in the history of modern methods of training, as it shows, supported by practical results, what can be achieved by co-operation between laboratory and workshop 2 . Ill Analysis of the Graphs When the graphs have been recorded by the methods described above, they have to be analysed mathematically. Perpendiculars are dropped from points at the beginning and end of each movement on the line recording strokes on to the time scale, from which the duration of each movement can be read off in hundredths of a second (see fig. 8). The period 1 Ch. DELLION : Tu seras dactylographe. Paris, Editions « Graphica », 24, rue de Bondy. 2 A recent application of these laboratory researches has been made by Mr. J. H. Estoup, who in his treatise on typewriting evolves from these experiments a system of fingering and a method of training. (J. H. E S T O U P : Métatype. Paris, 20, rue Gassendi.) — 22 — of contact with each key, thus found, is entered in column 2 of the tables, and the period of each interval, if between two strokes with opposite hands, is entered in column 4, and if between two non-alternate strokes (i.e. with two fingers of the same hand) in column 5. The average deviation (cols. 6 and 7) is then calculated by taking the arithmetic average of the differences between the duration of each interval and the average duration of that class of interval (i. e. alternating or nonalternating). The average deviation is an index of regularity. Needless to say, this analysis proved very difficult, owing to the strain on eyesight and attention caused by the number and minute detail of the signs recorded. The procedure adopted in order to avoid errors was as follows: the investigators first made the calculations, which were then done twice independently by outsiders. The results were compared, and only when all the figures arrived at agreed were they accepted as correct. This method of recording and calculation makes it possible to measure both the period for which the key is depressed and the period elapsing between the striking of two keys. Each of these elements in the manipulation of the keys will be considered separately. IV . Results P E R I O D OF DEPRESSION OF K E Y S When a first-class typist is working at full speed, every key is not held down for exactly the same period ; it varies from two to six hundredths of a second. The standard sentence employed for this study, Nous sommes en possession de votre lettre, contains 41 signs, including the space bar for separating words, and 40 intervals. Table I gives the analysis of the graph of one of the subjects, table II that of a graph recorded under the same conditions eight years later for the same subject, who had not pursued his occupation for several years, having been called up for military service. — 23 — It is unnecessary here to reproduce the actual graph recorded, as was done in the preceding experiment (Fig. 8); throughout the remainder of this report only the numerical analyses will be given and conclusions based on them. The unpublished graphs are preserved among the records of the Laboratory of Experimental Psychology at the "Ecole des Hautes E t u d e s " , and may, if required, be published. The results shown in the two tables are similar, except for a difference of 32 hundredths of a second in the total duration of the test, and variations in the length of the strokes and intervals. Variations of the same character had already been noted with the same subject in successive tests made in 1912 and quite recently. The increased length of the period of contact of each stroke, however, is probably due to an improvement in the method of recording. As the resistance of a brass rod is by no means negligible, whereas that of mercury is, it is not surprising that the duration of contact should be slightly longer and the intervals slightly shorter, as they proved to be. Careful analysis of the data contained in tables I and II reveals facts which will subsequently form the subject of more minute experiments. In the first test, the distribution of strokes by period was as follows: N u m b e r of strokes 2 15 16 8 Duration ('/ioo second) 5 4 2 2 Average 3.26 In the second experiment it was as follows: Number of strokes 7 11 11 7 5 Average Duration (Vioo second) 6 5 4 3 2 4.2!) The period of depression of certain keys varied from the average by four and three hundredths of a second in one case and three and five hundredths in the other, and it was desired to find the reason of this. The keys struck were therefore grouped according to the period of depression (table III). It was expected to find a relation between this period and the position of keys on the keyboard. No definite answer to this TABLE I. PERIOD OF CONTACTS A N D INTERVALS; GRAPH 84 Intervals Key struck (1) n 0 u s Space bar s 0 m m e s Space bar e n Space bar P 0 s s e s s i 0 n Space bar d e Space bar V 0 I r e Space bar / e 1 1 r e Period of contact C/.00 second) (2) 4 3 4 3 4 4 3 3 4 3 4 4 3 3 4 4 4 3 5 3 3 2 2 4 3 5 4 3 4 2 4 2 2 4 3 3 3 2 2 2 3 Hand used (3) i R R R L R L R R R L L R L R R R R L L L L L R R R R L L R L R R R L R R L R R R L Period ('/ ,oo second) Opposite hands (4) — 2 7 8 4 — — • 3 — 4 9 2 — — — 3 — — — — 5 — — — 6 — 3 8 3 — — 4 4 — 4 5 — — 3 Deviation C/,00 second) Same hand (5) Opposite hands (6) Same hand (7) 15 11 — 4.10 0.10 — — — — 2.57 2.43 3.43 0.57 — — 3.10 1.90 — — — 1.57 — 1.90 — — — 0.57 4.43 2.57 0.90 1.10 5.90 — — — — 1.57 2.90 3.10 3.10 2.90 — — — — — 0.43 1.90 0.90 1.90 — — — — 1.43 2.10 — — — — 1.57 3.43 1.57 1.10 2.10 — — — — 0.57 0.57 — 3.10 — — 0.57 0.43 2.90 1.10 — — — 1.57 14 9 9 10 12 5 8 14 14 8 9 10 9 13 12 13 14 8 12 — Total 87 134 229 Duration of the test 4 50 Average period of the 41 contacts 3.26 Average of the 19 intervals between strokes with opposite hands 4.57 — Average of the 21 intervals between strokes with the same hand 10.90 — General average of intervals 7. 90 — — — — — — — — — — — — — — — 31.85 48.10 — — 1.67 — — — 2.29 — , TABLE II. PERIOD OF CONTACTS AND INTERVALS ; GRAPH 224 Intervals Key struck Period of contact C/.00 Hand used second) a) (2) (3) 71 0 11 S 6 5 2 6 5 6 3 3 4 5 6 4 5 3 5 4 5 4 6 5 4 5 3 4 2 5 6 6 4 3 4 3 2 5 4 5 4 2 3 2 4 R R R L R L R R R L L R L R R R R L L L L L R R R R L L R L R R R L R R L R R R L Space bar s 0 m m e s Space bar e n Space bar P 0 s s e. s s i 0 n Space bar d e Space bar V 0 1 r e Space bar / e t I r e (Period »/ ,oo second) Deviation (i/ioo second) Opposite hands (4) Same hand (5) Opposite hands (6) 3 6 9 2 — — 2 — 4 8 2 — — — 4 — — .— — .4 — — — 6 — 4 6 3 — — 4 3 — 2 5 — — 3 15 10 — — .— — 13 12 — 8 .— — — 10 14 7 —6 12 13 6 — 11 10 11 — 8 — — — 13 10 — — 18 — — 12 13 — — 1.21 1.79 4.79 2.21 — — 2.21 — 0.21 3.79 2.21 — — — 0.21 — — — — 0.21 — — — 1.79 — 0.21 1.79 1.21 — _. 0.21 1.21 — 0.21 0.79 — —1.21 .— — — 2.17 1.17 — 2.83 — — — 0.83 3.17 3.83 — 4.83 1.17 2.17 4.83 — 0.17 0.83 0.17 — 2.83 — — — 2.17 0.83 — — 2.17 — — 1.17 2.17 — 27.27 44.51 — — 1.43 :— — — 2.02 — — 80 227 Total 173 Duration of the test 48 0 Average period of the 41 contacts 4.29 Average ol the IS) intervals between strokes with opposite hands 4.21 -— Average of the 21 intervals between strokes with the same hand — 10.80 General avera ge ot inte rvals 7.É / Same hand (7) 4.17 0.83 — 26 — question was obtained, but one fact was noted, namely, that strokes with the left hand were longer than those with the right. TABLE I I I . RELATIVE P E R I O D OF DEPRESSION OF D I F F E R E N T K E Y S PERIOD (seconds) 1 KEYS STRUCK 6/1OO 5/1OO 4/IOO S/100 s- LU © 0 © ® (2 0 S O S 2 (H S 2/1OO Underlined ® D 1 = struck with right hand. struck with left hand. alternation before and alter, alternation after. alternation before. Subject: D ; test given in 1920. Table IV, containing the results of 14 tests with six subjects, tends to show that this fact may be treated as a generalisation. TABLE IV. DURATION OF STROKES W I T H T H E L E F T AND RIGHT HANDS RESPECTIVELY Contacts Subject Number Right | A — — B C B A — — D E — — F 20 20 20 20 25 20 20 20 20 25 20 20 20 20 Total period (V100 second) Average per stroke (V100 second) Left Right Left Right 21 21 21 21 16 21 21 21 21 16 21 21 21 21 82 87 96 86 116 92 100 97 102 93 99 99 102 147 105 106 128 106 77 107 122 120 1.17 80 139 1.44 136 184 4.10 4.35 4.80 4,30 4.64 •1.60 5.00 4.85 4.90 3.72 4.95 4.95 5.10 7.35 Number of graph Left 5.00 5.04 6.09 5.04 4.81 5.09 5.80 5.71 5.76 5.00 6.61 6.85 6.47 8.76 238.1 238.2 234.3 238 90 238 234.1 234.4 234.2 214 234-B 224 224-B 303 — 27 — DURATION OF INTERVALS NECESSITY OF ALTERNATING THE HANDS The analysis of graphs given in tables I and II shows definitely the conditions determining rapidity in the intervals between strokes; this point is brought out in columns 4 and 5 of the tables. It will at once be seen that when two letters are struck with different hands the interval is much shorter than when they are struck with the same hand. The averages bring this out very clearly. First test . . . . Second test . . . Alternating strokes Non-alternating strokes General, average 4.52 4.21 10.90 10.80 7.71 7.67 These experiments, frequently repeated with very different subjects, always produced similar results. Take, for instance, three other typists, varying widely both among themselves and from the first (D), whose performance has already been recorded. C is a well known champion typist, who uses only one finger of each hand. G is a woman, slightly less skilled than the two preceding, but an excellent typist. H is also an expert, and though not a champion has the reputation of being one of the best typists in the profession. Detailed analysis of their tests, on the lines shown in tables I and II, confirms the fact noted above. If the results of the analyses are grouped in a single table, it will at once be seen that the intervals are always shorter when the hands are used alternately (table V). For some of the subjects the results of two tests are given, in order to show (1) that the same experiment repeated under absolutely identical conditions — as in the case of G — does not give the same numerical results, a fact to be explained by the instability of the human psychomotor system, which in itself constitutes a capacity for improvement; (2) that, whatever numerical variations may be noted with the same subject, the rule as to alternation of the hands is always confirmed. — 28 TABLE V. DURATION OF STROKES A N D INTERVALS, IN H U N D R E D T H S OF A SECOND, FOR FOUR SUBJECTS Average period Subjects in order of speed C D (1913) (1921) G (1913) (1921) H Period of test Strokes Intervals between alternating strokes 411 450 480 536 525 550 4.58 3.26 4.29 6.24 6.04 3.12 4.26 4.57 4.21 6.00 6.38 7.00 Average deviation Intervals between Nonnon-alter- Alternating alternating nating strokes 6.76 10.90 10.80 7.47 7.25 13.17 1.06 1.67 1.43 1.07 1.48 1.52 1.70 2.29 2.02 1.82 2.82 2.09 The facts mentioned above are even more strikingly shown by another graph (Fig. 10), in which the strokes and the intervals between them are the abscissae and the time in hundredths of a second the ordinates. Two curves are then drawn, one (continuous line) denoting the fluctuations in the length of the intervals, the other (dotted line) the fluctuations in the period of the strokes. Intervals between two non-alternating strokes are marked on the intervals curve by small circles. Strokes with the left hand are marked on the other curve by a cross. It will be noted that the two curves differ, that of the intervals fluctuating more than the other. The greatest fluctuations are due to non-alternation, and the contact curve shows less dispersion than the former. Strokes with the left hand generally occur on the upper portion of the curve. My most recent enquiries, using the most accurate methods with a large number of some of the most rapid professional typists, have served to extend and confirm the first results obtained. It should be noted that the subjects, though all first-class typists, employ different methods, using 4, 6, or 10 fingers. The results are shown in table. VI, to which are added data supplied by the tests with a subject who uses the tenfinger method almost exclusively, and who. though not a frequent competitor in championships, may be regarded as a first-rate typist. On the other hand, subjects like C, who has often won championships, and H, who enjoys a great reputation in the profession, both use two fingers only, and yet appear equally efficient, although using different methods. It would __ ^> Sui TJ e \ —•—• **~"»-^ r> ) .,'' <¿. —>—. *- • ^ ^ -'•* N -C o p*^' \ e — . —— — >-~ "— r s / ' ^ > ? o a s v -v ">., ^ =- \ -^ :?* =- \ =~ \ ^ 1^: - , ^ .-' / ( O h ( 0 i / l ' t i O t \ | - O Û ) i O l > o m m e í \ s pi .«¿= 0 v i D i n ^ l O N o •e TABLE VI. ANALYSIS OF GRAPHS OF THE TOUCH O Subject Period of test C/,00 second) (1) (2) A B A C A D E G H F H 386 387 395 401 404 407 411 417 417 450 480 510 510 525 536 550 570 589 Average t o t a Average duration of duration contact and of contacts interval : C/.oo second) index of speed ('/«»second) (4) (3) 9.35 9.38 9.75 9.69 9.77 9.92 10.09 10.14 10.14 11.16 11.96 11.96 12.37 12.97 13.17 13.20 13.39 14.18 4.53 4.70 4.31 4.16 5.39 5.39 4.55 5.46 5.34 3.26 4.29 5.56 5.80 6.00 6.24 3.12 8.07 3.02 Average duration of interval C/,w second) Alternating strokes (5) 3.64 3.58 3.88 3.70 3.17 3.47 4.26 3.76 3.88 4.57 4.21 4.76 5.11 6.38 6.00 7.00 3.31 8.11 Non-alternating strokes (6) 6.00 5.78 6.60 6.56 5.60 5.60 6.76 5.60 5.73 10.90 10.80 8.04 8.04 7.25 7.47 13.17 7.17 14.21 Ave (7 4 4. 5. 5. 4. 4 5 4, 4 7 7 6 6 6 6 10 5 11 — 31 — seem, therefore, that the common elements in the touch of all these subjects, as recorded by my system, is in fact characteristic of the vocational physiology of a typist. At the beginning of this report it was affirmed that the data obtained from first-class typists could be generalised. This hypothesis had to be confirmed; subjects with a good knowledge and practical experience of typewriting, though slow, were therefore tested. The results confirm those of previous tests. One subject, for example, was naturally slow in movement, his slowness being almost pathological; he strikes the keys as slowly as it is possible to conceive. This person is fifty years of age and has been a typist for twenty years; though such a slow operator he is useful in the office where he is employed owing to his scrupulous accuracy. Even in a semi-pathological case like this, the rule of alternation is confirmed. Average time of contact Alternating intervals Non-alternating intervals Hundredths of a second 4.75 24.90 27 .90 All the results obtained therefore confirm the theory that alternation of the hands promotes speed in typewriting. CONFIRMATION OF R U L E OF ALTERNATION NON-ALTERNATING PHRASES AND ISOLATED LETTERS A rule to which there are no exceptions in ordinary sentences nevertheless has to be checked by control experiments. For this purpose special tests were given, consisting of sentences typed without alternating the hands, i. e. entirely with one hand. Sentence Typed with the Right Hand. The sentence chosen was: CO, hop là, mon joli moujik, which all typists, whatever method they follow, type entirely with the right hand. The test was given after the subjects had mastered this somewhat peculiar sentence, and the unfamiliar — 32 — one-hand method involved. The results of analysing the graphs, as was done for the other sentences, were as follows: TABLE VII. RESULTS OF TYPING SENTENCE WITH T H E RIGHT H A N D ONLY Subject Average period of contact (Vioo second) Average period of intervals ("1W second) Total: index of speed ( V i c second) Number of graph (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) B A D — G F 4.60 5.68 3.76 6.07 6.92 6.64 8.25 9.00 14,51 14.07 15.95 16.33 12.85 14.68 18.27 20.14 22.87 22.97 235 235 214-4 87-1 88 303 All the subjects were excellent typists, the first two of the highest class. They were ranked according to their score in column 4 (the sum of the averages in columns 2 and 3). It will be noted that this ranking corresponds exactly to one made on the basis of the average period of the intervals, which would seem to show that speed is attained rather by shortening the intervals than by shortening the period of contacts. Sentence Typed with the Left Hand. A similar method was adopted with the sentence Restez vers cette terre, which is ordinarily typed entirely with the left hand, except for the space bar, which is always struck with the right. The results, which are given in table VIII, were what was expected. Here also the ranking according to the sum of the average interval and contact coincides with one based on the average period of intervals alone, except for the two best performers, who were always very close together and considerably ahead of their competitors, although the latter were also excellent typists. — 33 — TABLE VIII. RESULTS OF T Y P I N G WITH THE LEFT Subject B A G D HAND SENTENCE ONLY Average period of intervals (V100 second) Average period of contact O/ioo second) Nonalterna ting Alternating Average 5.21 5.86 5.86 7.08 5.04 6.06 5.87 6.93 9.87 1-1.81 5.50 4.83 8.00 8.66 13.16 5.76 Ó.35 7.46 9.26 14.36 Average total period: index of sjiced ('/,.. Number of graph second) 10.97 11.27 13.32 16.34 19.40 235 235 88-D 88-C 214-3 The experiments might be completed by tests with a sentence in which the hands are used in regular alternation, but it is difficult to find one complying with these conditions. The space bar, which is always struck with the right hand, makes complete alternation impossible — a difficulty which also arose, as mentioned above, with the sentence typed entirely with the left hand. "With the alternating sentence it was thought that the difficulty could be ignored, since the alternation involved in striking the space bar in itself promoted speed, and to prove the greater is to prove the less. In this case, however, the interruption of the regular alternation frequently lessened speed, or, in other words, was unfavourable to the subject, and diminished the value of the tests. For this reason it was considered preferable to neglect it. Typing Two Isolated Letters. The rule of alternation was verified even more simply and accurately. Supposing this rule to be correct, if a single key, such as /, be struck repeatedly and as rapidly as possible and then another key, say /', in the same way, the speed so achieved will be. less for either key than if thekeys were struck alternately. Experiment on these lines fully confirmed the rule, as is shown below. 3 — 34 — TABLE IX. RESULTS OF STRIKING ISOLATED Average period (Vioo second) Key struck and finger used KEYS Total second) Number of graph 15.04 215-1 17.68 215-2 7.67 216-1 (7.00 Contacts Intervals f (first finger, left) j (first finger, right.) 5.60 2.40 9.14 15.28 Alternation : /•(first finger, left) j (first finger, right) 3.60 4.07 /'(first finger, left) Space bar (first finger, right) 5.04 3.59 8.63 215-3 Non-alternation: j (first finger, right) Space bar (first finger, right) 3.66 11.35 15.01 215-4 j (first finger, right) Space bar (right thumb) 4.84 17.88 22.72 215-5 The first three lines in the table show that alternation shortens the duration of the movements by 50 or even 75 per cent. The size of this proportion, greater even that than produced by previous experiments, is due to the fact that this test brings into play only the most elementary forms of nervous activity. The fourth line of the table shows that the letters / and / do not constitute a special case, since alternation with the space bar (a part of the machine offering greater resistance) also produces greater speed. The figures, in the fifth and sixth lines confirm these data, showing also that it is definitely better to strike the bar with the index finger than with the thumb, as generally recommended. This, however, needs to be verified by more numerous experiments. Typing Short Isolated Words. These experiments can be extended to groups of letters forming ordinary words, which occur very frequently in typewriting. Table X, for instance, gives the results of analysing a graphic record of typing the word avec. — 35 — The first line gives the result where the word was typed on the ten-finger system, i.e. a — fourth finger, left. v — first finger, left. e — second finger, left. c — second finger, left: space bar — right t h u m b . a — fourth finger, left, etc. There are two alternate for every three non-alternate movements; and the rule is verified. A more rational method of typing, based on the principle of alternation, would be as follows : a — fourth finger, left. v — first finger, right. e — second finger left. c — first finger, right. space bar — first finger, right. a — fourth finger left, etc. This gives four alternate movements to one non-alternate; the results given in the second line of the table show that speed is increased 50 per cent. TABLE X. RESULTS OF T Y P I N G ISOLATED WORDS Word and method of t y p i n g Contacts C/.00 second) Intervals ('/ioo second) Alternating Nonalternating C/,00 second) Number of graph Total avec (ten fingers), alternating with space bar only 5.60 12.77 15.73 28.50 218,1 avec, maximum alternation 3.69 4.77 9.28 14.05 218,2 vers (ten fingers), alternating with space bar only 5.46 8.42- 12.08 20.50 218,3 vers, maximum alternation 3.35 4.35 13.28 17.63 218,4 lettre (ten fingers), 2 alternations with space bar only 4.71 6.87 15.42 22.20 218,5 lettre, alternating 4.07 5.80 11.18 16.98 218,6 — 36 — If another word, vers, is typed according to the two methods (table X), the totals show the superiority of the alternating method. In this experiment the total length of each test was compared taking into account the same number of signs in each case; the first test took 621 hundredths of a second, the second only 359 hundredths. Finally a longer word, lettre, typed according to the two methods (table X), again provides confirmation of the rule. These experiments were made with a subject, who uses the six-finger method, but had previously practised a long time before, typing with ten fingers. For definite conclusions, however, the experiments would have to be repeated. RELATION BETWEEN PERIOD OF CONTACTS, PERIOD OF INTERVALS, AND FORCE OF STROKE The period of contacts and that of intervals have hitherto been studied separately; it now remains to consider the relation between these two factors in the speed of typewriting. If, in table VI, the. figures for the period of contacts are subtracted from those for intervals, both positive and negative results are obtained in a single test. A B D +0.29 — 0.02 — 1.01 — 0.86 — 0.78 — 0.54 + 0.93 + 0.97 +4.64 + 3.38 E G H C F — 0.84 — 0.79 + 0.97 + 0.53 +6.95 + 8.13 + 0.93 — 2.61 Other experiments produced similar results, but those given above are sufficient for present, purposes. It will be. noted that the subjects fall into three classes: (1) those whose intervals are longer than their contacts, i.e. B, D, G, H, C; (2) those whose intervals are the shorter, i.e. E, F ; (3) mixed types, such as A.; these are rare. There, are clearly marked individual differences in each group, for example, among B, D, and H, from which a motor characteristic can often be deduced, though it cannot be related to — 37 — special efliciency. Though D, B, and H employ different methods, all turn out excellent work. Investigations now in progress, however, may, it is hoped, establish a relation between this characteristic and the rhythm of strokes which could be used in training typists. The question must also be studied from another point of view, to ascertain how these two factors vary when speed increases through alternation of the hands. When there is no alternation whatever (see table VII), the speed index of a single subject increases by one-third, if not more (table XI), wliich denotes a corresponding decrease in typewriting speed. The next question is to what extent the two factors in this index — period of contact and period of interval — are modified by alternation of the hands. Roth factors are decreased. If a new element of comparison be introduced, consisting of a left-handed test in which the necessity for striking the space bar with the right hand causes occasional alternation, it will be noted that the period of the intervals varies with changes in the index of speed, but that of the contacts does not do so. TABLE X I . VARIATIONS IN S P E E D I N D E X , CONTACTS, AND I N T E R VALS, WITH COMPLETE ABSENCE OF ALTERNATION (TEST 1 ) , OCCASIONAL ALTERNATION (TEST 2 ) , AND EQUAL D I S T R I B U TION OF ALTERNATION AND NON-ALTERNATION (TEST 3 ) Subject 1 Speed index C/ioo second) Period of intervals 0/,oo second) Period of contacts (7,00 second) Test Test Test 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 B 12.45 10.97 9.69 8.25 5.76 5.13 4.60 5.21 4.31 A 14.68 11.27 9.35 9.00 5.35 4.82 5.68 5.86 4.53 D 20.14 19.40 11.16 14.57 14.36 7.90 3.76 5.04 3.26 6.79 6.92 7.08 6.24 G 22.87 16.34 12.97 15.95 9.26 The above table, which gives a few figures illustrating these points, shows that in the second test the period of the contacts even varies in inverse ratio. This cannot, however, be treated as a generalisation, as opposite results were obtained in several — 38 - cases. It can simply be stated that speed in typewriting is mainly achieved by shortening the intervals; shortness of contacts would seem to be an effect rather than a cause of speed. This point required further investigation. The third factor in touch, namely, the force used in striking a key, is also related to the period of the contacts and intervals. Alternation of the hands immediately reduces all the factors in movement: period of contact, intervals, and strength of touch. This is best shown by pneumatic recording machines. One of the subjects, C, having stated that he had been struck by the ease with which he typed the word monsieur, a graph of this word as typed by him was accordingly made with a pneumatic instrument. This graph (Fig. 11) shows that the first three letters, which are struck with the same hand, are typed relatively slowly, both contacts and intervals being comparatively long; the last five letters, for which the hands alternate regularly, are typed with great speed; the intervals are so short that the curves for the strokes almost run into one another, and the contacts so short that the recording needle runs less than half as high as for the first three letters, which shows that the force of the strokes decreased by half. The first three letters were typed in 40 hundredths of a second, end the last five in only 29hundredths. — 39 — PHYSIOLOGICAL REASONS FOR ALTERNATION OV THE HANDS The standard technique of typewriting when investigated was observed to conflict with the result of laboratory experiments, which should scientifically provide the. basis of all training. Enquiry into the cause of this error showed that it arose from the false analogy commonly made between the technique of the piano and that of the typewriter. The pianist produces a melody by contacts and intervals of varying length which is strictly determined by the composer. A typist, on the other hand, should not consider such matters; his movements must be rapid and as uniform as possible. The keys of a piano may be played either legato or staccato, whereas in typing each stroke must always be distinct. In piano playing harmonies are formed by striking several notes simultaneously, the left hand being used largely to play chords as an accompaniment to the air. The technique of typewriting is entirely different, for in this case to strike two keys at once is a serious fault. The keyboard of a piano is long and narrow and the hand must therefore be spread as widely as possible. The keyboard of a typewriter, consisting of four parallel lines of keys, is much more compact; it does not call for spreading the hand, but in strict logic should require, lengthening the fingers, which is absurd. For all these, reasons, there is a need for a technique of typewriting entirely different from that of the piano. It is unnecessary here to pass any final judgment on the tenfinger method, which is based on a false analogy between the typewriter and the piano, but it may be noted that the majority of typewriting pupils practise for months and years in order to achieve difficult feats, for which they have, no aptitude, simply because this method has become orthodox. Moreover, the same fate befalls typewriting methods as any other form of constraint on the working of the human organism imposed either by prejudice or in the name of some half-science: physiology always reasserts its rights. When, at. the height of the craze for Taylorism. I ventured severely to criticise the work of the celebrated American engineer 1 , I encountered more 1 J. M. L A H Y : Le système Taylor et la physiologie sionnel. Paris, Gauthier-Villars. du travail profes- — 40 — hostility than experimental criticism of an unduly rigid method of typewriting would be likely to meet with today. The human organism tends to free itself from external constraint; defective training alone does not account for the fact that the majority of typists taught on the ten-finger system later automatically take to using six, four, or even two fingers, as best suits their own organism. This was attributed to deliberate slackness on their part, and pupils were still compelled painfully to learn the orthodox system of fingering. Yet it would have been easy to make the simple experiment of tapping rapidly with one hand or the other, and then with the two hands alternately; this would have shown that much greater speed was attained when the hands were alternated. This phenomenon has long been studied in laboratories of experimental psychology by means of tapping tests. There are two methods of recording taps, pneumatic and electric. With the pneumatic method the subject taps with one or more finger tips on a semi-rigid membrane covering a closed wooden box. In a small opening in the box is inserted a rubber tube-connected with a Marey recording drum; thus each tap can be recorded on a revolving cylinder. With the electrical method the subject wears a metal thimble or holds a special pen with which he taps as rapidly as possible on a copper plate. The plate, the pen (or thimble), and a Desprez signal are placed in an electric circuit. When the disc is tapped with the pen the circuit is closed, and it is broken as soon as contact ceases, the Desprez signal recording each tap on a revolving cylinder. The time in fifths of a second is also recorded with a Jacquet chronograph. The typewriter equipped for experiments as described above also provided a convenient tapping test and was used for this purpose. It is in many respects better than other appliances, as alternation of the fingers as well as the hands can be recorded. It should be noted, however, that the results obtained by different methods cannot be compared, particularly those obtained with a typewriter and those with the two tapping appliances described above, owing to the resistance of a mechanical system of transmission, which is a negligible factor in_electric or pneumatic tapping instruments. Except where otherwise — 41 — stated, the results quoted here were obtained with electric tapping instruments. With these appliances graphs are obtained from which can be calculated motor rapidity, expressed by the number of taps per second during a 30-second test, and the coefficient of liability to motor fatigue, expressed by the formula n1 + n2 ~ r? where n 1 represents the first five seconds' tapping and n 2 the last five. Table X I I shows all the results obtained by analysing the tapping tests of a group of typists who assisted in the experiments during the International Championship at the Grand Palais in Paris. The table gives the figure for motor rapidity and the coefficient of liability to motor fatigue of each subject. It will at once be noted that alternation promotes speed. The figures given in previous tables and in table XII do not refer to exactly similar phenomena, as the former indicate the length of the intervals between two strokes in typewriting, the latter the number of experimental taps per second ; nevertheless the direction in which the figures vary indicates the relation between them. In elementary movements such as tapping (table XII) alternation increases motor speed by about onethird; this increase corresponds to that already noted in actual typewriting. It would appear, therefore, that alternation in typewriting is a primary phenomenon, since it is based on a physiological characteristic as elementary as that involved in the tapping test. If this be the case, the speed attained by experienced operators constitutes a limit which they cannot exceed. Several facts can be cited in proof of this. When a subject is asked to type as fast as possible, but to maintain a regular rhythm, he instinctively bases this rhythm not on the shortest intervals, though these appear to favour automatic action, but on the longest. One subject who was accustomed to work rhythmically, as he has to cut stencils, involving greater pressure on the keys in ^ <¡ w o a H v> wo is o z § r ^ > » ^ Subject r+ S -S H O G M > Z Ö SB S» Rapidity (taps per second) H > •0 Alternate (-' M H' l-l t 0 s ] 0 0 O 0 0 ! 0 00 0 l ( C K H 0 ! 0 O 0 3 O I » h i t D OCDCOOOOii-iis3 0 i t O C 5 < 1 0 0 0 * - i - * W * ' a 5 tsjt^OHJooi-' g O H O S) HI Left hand OrCnOi{J50iOiC50^05(rft05^.10i C0Or'r'*.O0l0l OltúO'tv>Oo4'-—lOiO**t\3>-'1í>-Ul>->OlOdi M *] Right band >^.^-ÜiOO*-WttiOOOO^.oo 4^ a Alternate + H Left hand + O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O i-'OriOO-'-'OK'LOMOOIO-'OlOh' 05MO)010MlOO)r'OPO)ai a o i - ' W B # . i \ : u o * . c o o M w o Method ! ooi r 1 OiOöaioooMfflSsoisi fingering ! en — 43 — order to pierce the wax used for making copies, typed the standard sentence, Nous sommes en possession de votre lettre, with the following results: Hundredths of a second Non-alternating strokes Alternating strokes 12.52 10.52 The average deviation of alternating and non-alternating strokes, previously 6.38 hundredths of a second, fell to 2 hundredths. This shows a clearly marked tendency towards regular rhythm if the longer intervals are taken as basis. The figures are as follows: Non-rhythmic periods Rhythmic periods Increase in periods Non-alternating strokes (Via, second) Alternating strokes C/ioo second) 10.90 12.52 4.52 10.52 1.62 6.00 Difference C/ioo second) 6.38 2.00 4.38 (tendency to rhythm) What is here called the tendency to rhythm, i.e. the difference between the figures expressing the variation in the period of the strokes (alternating or non-alternating), is merely a convenient method of ranking subjects from this point of view. An experiment with another subject gave the following results: Non-alterna ting strokes C/ioo second) Alternating strokes ('/ice second) C/ioo second) Non-rythhmic test Rhythmic test 13.04 14.13 6.82 7.94 7.22 6.19 Increase in period 1.09 1.12 — Tendency to r h y t h m Difference 1.03 In comparing the results for these two subjects, it will be seen that the ability to make typing movements rhythmic is not found to an equal degree in all typists; it is an individual characteristic which should be utilised in specialising for copying work. — 44 — Be this as it may, the periods in rhythmic tests were much the same as the longest ones in non-rhythmic tests, which shows that the greatest speeds are independent of deliberate rhythm and measure elementary physiological capacities. This hypothesis is confirmed by another fact. With the typewriter used as a tapping apparatus a record was taken of taps on two adjoining keys: d and ¿>, struck first with the left hand, then alternately with each hand. The subject of the experiment (D) tapped 6.13 times per second with one hand, and 11.66 times per second alternating the hands. Table X I I shows that in ordinary tapping the same subject tapped 6.12 times per second with the left hand and 10.33 per second with alternate hands. The figures recorded are thus of the same nature, and the phenomenon which they indicate appears to be practically incapable of reduction to more elementary factors. Without embarking on investigations or explanations in^ pure physiology, one is led to enquire what mechanism determines the limit of typewriting speed. The nervous and muscular factors involved when two successive strokes are performed with the same hand are probably the same in each movement. As these movements are very rapid, not exceeding a few hundredths of a second, the internal changes in the tissues which determine these movements occur during the periods of inertia in each movement of the hands when used alternately. All movements, however simple, are the result of a chemical process inside the nerve cells, to mention these only. When a cell functions it expends part of its reserves, and simultaneously a process of assimilation takes place to reconstitute these reserves. There is an incessant interchange of nutritive products and waste products; this is indicated by a substance called chromophil found in the interstices of the network formed by the cytoplasm of the nerve cell. When the balance of nutrition is disturbed, this substance disappears progressively, beginning at the nucleus, which itself moves from the centre of the cell towards its circumference, while the cell diminishes in volume. While the balance of nutrition in the cell is destroyed by excessive activity, the destruction of this minute organism is — 45 — hastened by another phenomenon. The waste products, the residue of the chemical processes which have taken place, are not eliminated sufficiently quickly and paralyse the cell's action like a poison. These two causes together will stop the action of a cell if there is not a sufficient interval between two successive stimuli or two reactions. There are several proofs of this. When the brain cells of a dog which has been trepanned are stimulated electrically, the motor reactions to this stimulus are only obtained if there is an interval of at least one-tenth of a second between each stimulation; the refractory period in animals is thus one-teuth of a second. The same is true of human beings. The special cells forming the retina under the action of light waves undergo a chemical process which produces visual sensation. If the successive sensations are to amalgamate, the intermittent stimulations causing them must strike the retina at intervals of less than one-tenth of a second. If the intervals are greater, the sensory cells start a new process and separate instead of fusing the successive images, for as each refractory period ends a new image is distinguished. As is well known, if a subject wishes to produce a series of voluntary muscular reactions, the maximum number is nine or ten per second for the most gifted persons under the most favourable conditions. But this limit is not the same in reactions produced by exciting the" muscle electrically without recourse to the nervous system ; in this case 40 separate shocks per second can be produced. The refractory period of the nervous cells is thus approximately one-tenth of a second, which means that if two stimuli are applied to one of these cells within a shorter period, the same phenomenon will occur as in the fusion of luminous impressions: the cell will not react to the second stimulation. Two things must at once be noted without which it would be impossible to explain individual differences in typewriting speed. In the first place, the figure of one-tenth of a second is not strictly accurate; there are probably individual variations which, though slight, are nevertheless characteristic. Secondly — and this is important — the movements of the various parts of the hand used to strike the keys involve more than one cell. It is probable that groups of cells fulfil this function, and though — 46 — they act almost together substitutions may be effected and increase the muscular action of the hand. Moreover, though the same hand may be used for striking, the same finger is not always used. The reciprocal action of adjacent cells in the same area of the nervous system is not known in detail, but it may legitimately be supposed that the use of different fingers involves different though adjacent cells1. Take as examples our two best subjects, A and B. A rearrangement of their results, as is done for one of them in table XIII, gives the following figures, which may be compared with their results in the tapping test. Time C/ioo second) Unit measured A Complete stroke, not alternating Tapping movement, not alternating Complete stroke, alternating Tapping movement, alternating 10.48 14.44 8.16 12.03 B 10.82 13.29 8.17 8.71 It will be seen that both subjects come very near the maximum rapidity which physiologists have endeavoured to define. The favourable effects of the position assumed in typewriting and of the subject's constant practice in this position are clearly shown by the fact that here' the electric tapping is slower than the typewriting movements. The tapping test clearly shows that striking alternately with either hand, using nerve cells situated on opposite sides of the system and acting independently of each other, produces very much greater speed in the movements as a whole. This explains the benefit derived from alternating the hands in striking the keys. There is too often a tendency to explain apparently simple physiological phenomena by facts which themselves are too simple. It is my aim to avoid this mistake. To attribute the .*. An example of substitution is afforded by the muscles used in striking the keys of a typewriter. The movement of the finger muscles is localised to varying degrees in different subjects ; often the entire hand and sometimes the arm are involved. W h a t happens in the muscles between the shoulder and the finger tips must also happen in the nerve cells involved to varying degrees in the act of striking the keys. — 47 — TABLE XIII. RELATION BETWEEN PERIOD OF CONTACTS AND INTERVALS WITH AND WITHOUT ALTERNATION OF T H E H A N D S 1 Successive strokes 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Total Average Total Contacts Intervals 4 4 5 5 4 5 5 4 5 6 4 5 5 4 3 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 3 4 4 4 6 5 5 3 4 4 5 7 5 2 6 3 5 5 10 5 4 4 6 2 3 7 1 9 3 7 3 5 5 3 3 6 9 8 6 2 8 6 9 1 7 3 6 1 7 4 4 3 6 6 2 8 4 4 180 200 — — « Subject: A (grapli.No. 238). Alternating Nonalternating — 14 9 9 9 10 7 — — 6 — 7 12 8 — — — 7 — — — — 7 — -— — 5 — 8 11 4 11 — — 10 — — — — 8 11 — 15 — — — 9 8 7 — 10 14 13 11 — It 10 13 — 13 — — — — 8 9 — — — — — 11 — 8 11 9 9 139 241 — 8 8.16 10.48 — 48 — superiority of certain typists over others entirely to the rapidity of their nervous processes is, I am well aware, an easy solution. I have pointed out elsewhere that memory, spelling, and various other mental functions are factors of superiority which, combined in varying degrees in different typists, lead to superior efficiency1. But apart from the fact that such functions are very different from the motor functions just discussed, they do not in themselves explain why highly skilled operators are not exactly the same in their exercise of the profession. Reverting to the examples of A and B, who take the highest places in international competitions, it will be noted that A shows greater regularity than B in the same tests. Can experimental psychology furnish an explanation of this difference ? A partial explanation is already given by the tapping tests of these two subjects. In simple measurement of motor rapidity B is superior to A, but when the tapping test is in typewriting, and not on experimental appliances, Ais more rapid. To explain this the reaction times of both subjects were carefully analysed. Their reaction times were measured in a series of tests, which gave the following significant figures. Significant figures A B Calculated from all the observations. (1) Arithmetic average (A) (2) Mean deviation (D) (3) Index of variability given by the formula 15 1.4 15.33 2 100 1) A 9.3 Calculatedfrom observations between the quart iles, i.e. excluding extremes. (4) Median (m) (5) Semi-interquartile -i(6) Index of variability given by the formula i _ 2 X 100 m 15 1 6.8 13 15 ( + 0.03) 1.5 9.3 1 J. M. LAHY : " La méthode à suivre pour la sélection des travailleurs : les dactylographes ", in Anals de l'Institut d'orientacio professional, Vol. II, No. 3, pp. 65-78. Barcelona. — 49 — These figures show that t h e two subjects are normal and of average speed, but A is the quicker of the two. The most typical differences are shown by Galton's ogive (Figs. 12 and 13). S c r1 s -S Vlédian ,14 7 1 s L Moyenne >15 i l l 1 N u m b e r of reaction times in srder of magnitude Frequency F I G . 12. Distribution curve of reaction times, subject A. 1 s n • r ,-J 1 u r Frequency ¿ Médian M. .3 Moyenne = 15.33 l 1I 1 N uml j e r of reeict ioi i t im es in 01 de r (>f nai mit lid e F I G . 13. Distribution curve of reaction times, subject B . With A the mode is clearly defined, there being little dispersion. The series is homogeneous, the averages are definite, and outlying observations rare; the curve shows that the reactions are regular and homogeneous. With B the mode is less clearly defined, the curve has a greater range, and outlying observations are more frequent. A is less variable than B, their indices of variability being in the proportion of about 7 to 10 for the normal range and 9 to 13 for the total range. A is also superior in motor ability, as she taps more quickly and varies less. These facts are brought out even more clearly by the chronological record of the observations. (Figs. 14 and 15). In spite of his greater speed in elementary functions (tapping), B is less well equipped in motor ability for typewriting owing to the greater instability of his motor functions. The results — 50 — of testing reaction times thus agree with those of professional efficiency. « Mean F I G . 14. Chronological record oí reaction times, subject A. 22 21 20 19 18 j i\ / • 7 -C ) 16 15 — \ 14 13 12 '1 {^ | H?F s1 \ IL v V 11 \ -X. \1 \ r\\ \ \ 1 H 1 f / —(t-tl- -j / / '" — Vlean M )-<.> V• > F I G . 15. Chronological record of reaction times, subject B . These remarks, which might appear in a study of professional superiority, are inserted here to show that, though physiology can furnish explanations for the necessity of alternatingthe hands, it also teaches that its rules are not yet as rigid as the laws of physics. Physiology is characterised by the diversity and multiplicity of its working in any given subject; the application of rules based on physiological observation therefore produces less uniform results than that of rules based on observations in physics. The individual differences noted between A and B thus explain why they were not always ranked in the same order, though their general efficiency was the same. — 51 — LIMITS TO THE R U L E OF ALTERNATION There would seem to be no exceptions to the rule as to the alternation of the hands; it is obvious, however, that when this involves moving both hands towards the extremities of the keyboard it is desirable to use the fingers of one hand, to avoid moving the other a considerable distance. Another reason for limiting the alternation of the hands is to avoid crossing them. For instance, in the word compartiment, if the hands are used in strict alternation, for the first syllable the left hand after striking the letter c would have to pass under the right hand, which strikes o, in order to type m; such movements would be complicated. U S E OF FINGERS OF ONE H A N D : "JUMPING " AND ALTERNATION "When alternation of the hands is limited as described above, various questions arise. How should the keys be struck with fingers of one hand ? Is it better to "jump " from one key to another with a single finger, or to alternate the fingers ? Using the method of recording already described, these questions may be answered by analysing the resulting graphs. The subject is first asked to strike, two adjacent keys in the same row, / and d, for example, as quickly as possible, with the same finger; he is then asked to strike the same keys very rapidly using two fingers of the same hand alternately. The results, given in table XIV, show that it is better to alternate the fingers. With two other keys, d and e, on different rows though adjacent, these results are confirmed (see third and fourth lines in the table). A new factor may be introduced by the use of the space bar, compelling alternation of the hands; it will be seen (line 5) that the speed is scarcely increased at all, in spite of the added assistance given. By combining alternation of the fingers with alternation of the hands, however, it will be seen (line 6) that the maximum speed is attained by combining the signs involved. If two keys in different rows and distant from each other, c and c, for example, are struck first by "jumping " the second finger from c to e, and then by using the second finger for e and the first finger for c, comparison again shows the superiority of the alternating method. — 52 — These experiments tend to show that for the left hand the fingers should be alternated. If this rule appears so clearly for the left hand, one might suppose that it would apply even more strictly to the right hand in right-handed persons, as the right hand is generally more dexterous, more frequently employed, and is often stronger. A similar experiment, however, gave a different result, as shown in the table. Alternation of the fingers of the right hand is thus not favourable to speed. TABLE XIV. RESULTS OF " J U M P I N G " AND ALTERNATING F I N G E R S OF T H E SAME HAND K e y struck and finger used Period ot contact C/lOO Period ot intervals CU Total C/.00 Number of second) second) second) graph 5.81 7.37 15.06 7.60 23.87 14.97 216,2 216,3 5.43 13.66 . 19.09 217,3 8.12 7.06 15.18 217,4 5.19 9.65 14.81 217,5 5.66 5.70 11.36 217,6 4.50 19.73 24.23 217,1 5.50 11.93 17.43 217,2 3.00 4.00 13.40 12.46 16.40 16.46 216,4 216,5 Keys in adjacent rows: j , u: jumping alternating 7.03 12.59 19.62 216,9 Keys in distant rows : ñ, a: jumping (first finger) alternating: n first, u second finger 2.68 11.93 14.61 217,7 3.12 15.93 19.05 217,8 Left Hand. Keys in one row: /', d: jumping alternating Keys in adjacent rows: rf, e : jumping (second finger) alternating: d first, e second finger jumping, with space bar alternating alternating: d first, e second finger (left), bar right t h u m b Keys in distant rows: c, e: jumping (second finger) alternating : c first, e second finger Right Hand. Keys in-one row: u, i: jumping alternating — 53 — These results proved somewhat surprising, and enquiries were made among a large number of typists to ascertain whether they regularly used methods involving the same movements for both hands. Here again, as with the. ten-finger method, it was found that hard and fast rules are not followed in practice. It was also noted that typists alternate the fingers of the right hand less than those of the left. This is perhaps the reason why my first enquiries into vocational efficiency tended to show that ambidexterity is a sign of greater efficiency. It may be noted in this connection that the greater number of keys most frequently struck are in the left half of the keyboard. There thus appear to be two kinds of movements, the efficiency of which varies. Hand movements are quicker and more skilful with the right hand than with the left; finger movements are simpler and quicker for the left hand than the right. These facts are confirmed by the prevailing practice. Typists questioned on this point agreed that it was easier to strike keys in the left half of the keyboard with the right hand than to strike a key in the right half of the keyboard with the left hand. The following rule may be deduced from these observations: with existing keyboards alternation of the fingers of the left hand and " j u m p i n g " the fingers of the right promote speed. RHYTHM IN TYPEWRITING The two time factors in striking the keys, period of contact and alternation, having been analysed, the function of rhythm in typewriting may now be considered. The solution of this problem would require a systematic study of touch, with variations in rhythm. This has not yet been undertaken, but an idea of the results obtainable may be formed by using the information already gathered in the course of my investigations. In typewriting with a good rhythm, the intervals should be as nearly equal as possible; the rhythm would be further improved if the period of contacts were also equal. Table XV shows the average deviation in the intervals recorded by a number of the subjects. It may be recalled that this figure is obtained by taking the differences between the general average of the intervals and the actual length of each interval, and calculating the average of these differences. — 54 — This average indicates the stability of the phenomenon under consideration. The nearer the average deviation is to zero, the greater is the regularity of typewriting and consequently the better the rhythm. TABLE XV. AVERAGE D E V I A T I O N OF A L T E R N A T I N G AND N O N ALTERNATING INTERVALS Average deviation Subject A — — — B F E D C G H Number of Alternating (Vioo second) Non-alternating C/.oo second) graph 1.10 1.16 1.21 1.77 1.66 1.19 1.18 1.17 1.24 1.54 1.38 1.67 0.06 1.26 1.48 1.52 1.00 1.72 1.88 1.37 1.86 2.13 1.82 1.33 2.58 1.58 2.10 2.19 1.70 1.72 2.82 2.09 234-3 238-6 234-4 234-2 238-5 234-1 238-1 238-4 303 224 224-5 84 90 88 89-2 85-1 It will be noted that the average deviation of the alternating intervals is less than that of the non-alternating, which means that alternation promotes regularity. But the question then arises whether it follows from this that in a scientific system typists should be required to adopt a rhythm in order to increase their speed - - a conclusion which seems warranted by the facts stated above. Although no very exhaustive enquiries were undertaken, a more or less fortuitous experiment may be cited, namely, cutting stencils. When a good typist cuts stencils, i. e. when each type, instead of being impressed on a sheet of paper, penetrates the waxed sheet used for the duplicator, he types more regularly but also more slowly. It appears that in any one subject speed is incompatible with regularity of rhythm. This can easily be — 55 — explained. In ordinary typing, speed is the main object; the typist makes the slight movements required in the minimum time necessary for repairing the wastage of the tissues where the changes on which the movements depend occur, as explained on an earlier page. In cutting stencils the typist directs his attention to each of his movements, watching them and regulating their force as he does not in ordinary typing. Part of his available energy is thus deflected by this additional effort. His movements are less automatic, every part of them is consciously observed, and they are harmonised in the simplest possible manner, by equalising their values. If two graphs for the same subject are compared, one made where the subject typed without considering the force, employed, the other when considering it, it will be observed that the rhythm is far more regular in the second case. The second fact to be noted is that, when two good typists are cutting stencils, the average deviation of the intervals, which, as already stated, indicates the regularity of their movements, is as follows: Average deviation of intervals Subject Alternating ('/,oo second) D G 1.52 2.35 Non-alternating (Vioo second) 1 .80 2.01 When the same subjects typed a sentence at full speed without stencils, the results were, as follows: Average deviation of intervals Subject D G Alternating (VIM second) Non-alternating (Y,oo second) 1.67 1.26 2.19 1.72 The complete reversal of the relative position of the two subjects calls for explanation. Before attempting to answer the — 56 - question, the average deviation of the reaction times of the two subjects was measured with a d'Arsonval instrument, with the following results: Subject Period ('/.oo second) Deviation (Vioo second) D G 13.90 18.53 1.30 2.27 It is probable that the subject with the best psychological equipment, i. e. whose reactions are quicker and more regular, loses his regularity in trying to work at high speed. His colleague, who works more slowly, is more stable. Experiments with reaction times call for very simple responses to sensory stimulation; but typing is différent. In this case the psychomotor system called into play is more complex, and owing to this complexity some individuals are able to evolve forms of reaction peculiar to themselves. In the first experiment the keys were struck as hard as possible in order to cut the stencil; in the second no force was required, and the subjects therefore worked as quickly as possible. Thus speed decreases as rhythm improves. It would be desirable to ascertain the relation between speed and force in striking the keys. This is at present the subject of investigation, which should provide fresh data for a scientific technique of typewriting. What is already known concerning rhythm in relation to the length of contacts and intervals justifies the assertion that rhythm imposed from without hampers speed. But it does not follow that, if a typist works in regular rhythm, either naturally or as the result of training, he cannot work quickly. On the contrary, rhythm added to other capacities may make its possessor a better typist than his fellows. The facts noted appear contradictory at first sight; there is a spontaneous tendency to regular rhythm in alternating strokes, which are the quickest; on the other hand, speed decreases when the typist is required to achieve regularity as well as speed. But these facts can easily be explained. In the first case, speed in typewriting tends to approach the physiological limits of speed in simple movements, which are essentially — 57 — regular; in the second, rhythm in more complex factors is sought: alternating and non-alternating strokes, contacts, intervals, and force. This involves considerable mental effort, which naturally limits speed. These observations do not solve the problem of rhythm in typewriting; they simply show that rhythm is not so important, for speed at any rate, as has been thought. At the very least the problem has been stated. V The Teaching of Typewriting SELECTION OF TYPISTS AND METHODS OF TRAINING All the investigations, both those described in this report and the much larger number of control experiments, have furnished two classes of data. There are first those which agree in all the experiments and may therefore be regarded as absolute; results of the second class, on the other hand, though frequently agreeing, are subject to certain exceptions and can only be regarded as of more or less general value. This should cause neither surprise nor scepticism concerning the value for professional purposes of the data supplied by scientific research. On the contrary, data of this kind should strengthen our convictions, as they confirm the common impression of the extreme diversity of human types. The conclusion to be drawn is that, though there are a few absolute rules which are of use in occupational work, there are many which can only be applied after more or less exhaustive examination of the subject — the typewriting pupil in this instance. The practical application of the results of these investigations will be based on this principle. The two factors in touch have been analysed by the graphic method; these are the period of contact with the keys, and the length of the intervals between these contacts — factors which were formerly indistinguishable. The relations between the two groups can now be established. — 58 — of Alternation ^ fundamental law, which is so general that it should govern of Hands improvements both in typewriting machines and in typewriting method, is the law of alternation of the hands. Both makers of typewriters and typewriting experts should endeavour to make alternation of the hands as frequent as possible. This law, it should be emphasised, is based, not on an individual opinion or on any technical necessity in the construction of typewriters, but on the working of the human organism. It cannot be ignored that, even though every attempt to improve typing technique should tend towards alternation of the hands, such efforts are necessarily limited. However the letters and signs are arranged on the keyboard of a typewriter, successive letters in any given word cannot always alternate, since the keyboard is fixed. No system of typing can secure perfect alternation of the hands. Consequently the fingers of the same hand must be employed successively. and'^jum'in " H ° w should the keys be struck with the fingers of the same of Fingers hand ? Should the fingers be alternated, or should one finger be " j u m p e d " ? Contrary to the prevailing belief, the answer is not the same for the two hands; it is a curious fact that the fingers of the more dexterous hand (usually the right) are less independent. In this case, therefore, " j u m p i n g " is preferable, whereas the fingers of the left hand should be alternated. The second fundamental rule of typing touch may therefore be stated as follows : alternation of the fingers of the left hand and " j u m p i n g " with those of the right promote speed. SCIENTIFIC ARRANGEMENT OF THE KEYBOARD The keyboards at present in use have been arranged on the basis of statistics as to the frequency with which letters occur in the words most used, the letters struck most frequently being placed in the most accessible parts of the keyboard. This principle is erroneous. The rule of alternation of the hands proves that the statistics used should be those of the. letters most frequently alternated and that these letters should then be placed in opposite parts of the keyboard. The space bar should no longer be regarded as a purely accessory appliance, placed outside the keyboard because it — 59 — works neither a letter nor a sign. On the contrary, it should be treated as an essential part of the keyboard; it should be included in statistics of the frequency of alternation and placed in the keyboard accordingly. The rectangular form of the keyboard also requires investigation. It might be advisable to make it in the form of a curve, more adapted to the fan-like arrangement of the fingers. Graphic analysis of the movements involved in typing would make a valuable contribution to the solution of the problem. Typing methods are based on the number of fingers used. of ,¿!Ie,( The theories formulated are generally based on certain facts which, though not always in themselves inaccurate, suffer from the common defect that they are based solely on observation uncontrolled by laboratory experiment. As general rules have been based on a single set of observations, it follows that they are not applicable to the majority of cases; hence the controversies between the partisans of the 2, 4, 6, 8, or 10 finger methods. It is a curious fact that one of these, the ten-finger method, is based on erroneous observations, and yet enjoys the greatest popularity and is the most frequently taught in typewriting schools. The error involved is the analogy drawn between typewriting and piano playing. The falsity of this analogy has already been explained, and it is unnecessary to revert to it. Graphic analysis of typewriting, as carried out above, leads to conclusions incompatible with the ten-finger method. According to physiological data, a scientific method should favour alternation of the hands; but the use of all ten fingers, in positions fixed with reference to each key, keeps the hands more or less motionless over each half of the keyboard (the fingers being used to strike the keys nearest their tips only) and decreases the frequency of alternation of the hands. The ten-finger method, having become the standard, is more minutely regulated than any other. Consequently it is less flexible and less adaptable to individual characteristics. The rules of this system, which are based on reasoning not always in harmony with the results of practical experience, cause waste of time in typewriting. For example, the two letters c and e, when situated in different rows of the keyboard, have — 60 — to be struck by "jumping " one finger. As shown in table XIV, this is an error, and it is better to alternate the fingers, striking c with the first finger and e with the second. It can also be shown that in the combination br it is better to strike b with the right first finger, and r with the left first finger, than to strike them both with the right first finger, "jumping ". One is therefore led to enquire whether the method which most favours alternation of the hands, i. e. the two-finger method, is not the best, as may logically be deduced from the first rule established above. Here again, however, practical experience is better than logic. It was shown that this method, rigidly applied, would cause waste of time owing to the excessive range of movement involved in striking two adjacent keys at one end of the keyboard after one hand had been used to strike a key at the other end. These statements may seem to make any positive practical conclusions impossible, but on the contrary they help to solve the problem of teaching typewriting. The ten-finger method, which is the least favourable to alternation of the hands, should not be imposed on pupils, but neither should the opposite two-finger method, for, though it gives the maximum of opportunity for alternating the hands, this advantage is limited by the arrangement of the keyboard. Tt must also be admitted that, though there are codified rules for the ten-finger method, there are no principles for teaching the two-finger system; this method is not taught, it is merely used by a few typists. The 4, 6, and 8 finger methods might also be criticised in the light of many experiments with individual cases, if their rules were as well defined as those for the ten-finger method. The impossibility of establishing a rigid method applicable to all typists has led to the general conclusion that typewriting touch is a personal matter, just like ordinary handwriting. There are very few people whose handwriting is the same; similarly typists are not built on identical lines, and cannot therefore produce identical results with the same methods. It has often been noticed, though hitherto insufficiently emphasised, that typists taught by the ten-finger method nearly always end by abandoning it. An enquiry conducted in a large typewriting school showed that only one-tenth of the pupils practised the ten-finger system, strictly though it was — 61 — taught. The teachers had to admit that after laborious training each pupil adopted his own method. Among typewriting champions are to be found some using two fingers, such as C, others six fingers, such as Y, A, and B, and very few, for example Z, ten fingers. These facts, however, must be accepted with reserve until the cinematographic investigations now in progress are concluded, as they alone can determine accurately the precise methods used by champions during record performances. The adoption of one or other method for teaching typewriting should depend on many factors. One of these is the anatomical structure of the hands and fingers (the ten-finger method presumably requires a large hand); the functional independence of the muscle groups which determine the agility of the fingers is also an important factor. But the essential factor in vocational characteristics, in my opinion, is to be sought in the more delicate anatomy, in the nervous system which determines motor rapidity. Take, for instance, an excellent subject, E, who uses the ten-finger method with great ease : the various tapping tests which he performed gave the following results: Fingers used Speed Liability. to fatigue I'irst finger, right First finaer, left First fìnger, left and right Left h a n d : first and second fingers first nnd third fingers first and fourth fingers 6.75 6.15 8.95 5.71 6.15 7.G8 0.18 0.16 0.24 0.10 — 0.08 Right hand: first and second fingers first and third fingers first and lourth fingers 9.70 10.07 9.80 0.22 0.19 0.25 Aptitude for alternation — 0.3.3 0.33 0.11 O.'M 0.11 — It will be observed that the motor rapidity of this subject is greater when the various fingers of the right hand are alternated with the first finger than when the hands are alternated. This is not so with the left hand, but the superiority of the right hand is enough to give this typist an advantage and confidence in the method he uses. It should also be noted that, thougli his — 62 — speed is less when the fingers of the left hand are alternated, the liability to fatigue is much less than with the right hand, or even with alternation of the hands. E is therefore well equipped for the ten-finger system. When subjects are not so equipped, it is useless to compel them to learn a method which they will not use in professional practice. Ea shouidbbeCt Ifc d o e s n o t follow that teaching should be left entirely to Taught the chance. To shorten the rperiod of trainingö and make it as Method . . effective as possible, the physiological characteristics of each candidate should first be determined. Tapping tests will be very useful here; the candidate's ability to alternate the various fingers can easily be ascertained and will show which method he should use. Everyone wishing to become a typist should therefore undergo an examination of this kind. When candidates have been selected by methods I have indicated elsewhere, they should be advised to adopt the method best suited to their natural abilities. tfaTypis°t" More exact knowledge of each typist will in future make it to the Machine possible to decide not only what method he should be taught, but also what machine he should use. As is well known, all typewriters are not equally reliable or equally rapid. As the greatest speed is attained by alternating the hands, it is simply necessary to make the typist strike one key with one finger of either hand alternately and as quickly as possible, and then to note if the letter is typed for every stroke. When a typist 'jams ' a given machine, his motor rapidity is thus known by means of this tapping test. By grading machines, as it were, learners could be given the type of machine best suited to them before they even began their training. The slowest subjects — and they too may make good typists — will achieve the maximum speed of which they are physiologically capable with slower machines. My investigations, which show that educationalists should adapt pedagogic rules to the individual conditions of the human organism, provide data applicable to the teaching of any kind of technique. To take one example: since alternation of the hands should be encouraged, pupils should no longer be required to treat the keyboard as divided in two portions, right and Suited to Him — 63 — left, but to endeavour to alternate the hands in typing every word. Mr. Dellion has already put this idea into practice in his rational typing system. Even the manufacture of typewriters should benefit from the data supplied by these experiments. Study of the period of contact of the fingers with the keys shows that this contact should be as brief as possible : for this purpose the .key should oñer the minimum of resistance, unless it also guarantees regularity of touch. Unlike the pianist, the typist should always strike the key rather than press it. The methods of applying the results of these investigations to the improvement of typing technique are far from exhausted, but my object is less ambitious. I have aimed rather to describe a method of studying occupational problems than to supply definite answers to every question which arises. This method may be defined as follows: every movement of the occupation is analysed by graphic, photographic, or cinematographic experiment, and from this analysis are deduced the best rules which workers should be advised to adopt in order to obtain the maximum output from any given tool. In conclusion one advantage of such a method may be indicated. It is easy to conceive the advantage which typewriting schools would derive from the possession of an apparatus like mine for investigating the touch of each of their pupils. The defects revealed by the graphic method could at once be rectified, and teaching methods could, when necessary, be modified to suit the physiological aptitudes thus revealed. The matter is of the utmost importance for recruiting and teaching typists, to judge by the number of these workers, the part they play in business life, and the importance of the industries which have been called into existence by typewriting.